An integrative model for the study of behavioral momentum.

J A Nevin
Author Information
  1. J A Nevin: Department of Psychology, University of New Hampshire, Durham 03824.

Abstract

Behavioral momentum is the product of response rate and resistance to change. The data on relative resistance to change are summarized for pigeons responding on single-key two-component multiple schedules, in the initial links of two-key multiple chained schedules, and in equivalent components of two-key serial schedules. For single-key procedures, the ratio of resistance to change in two schedule components is shown to depend on the ratio of reinforcer rates obtained in the presence of the component stimuli. For two-key procedures, the ratio of resistance to change in equivalent components is shown to depend on the ratio of reinforcer rates correlated with key locations. A model based on stimulus-reinforcer contingencies that combines the reinforcer rates in schedule components summed over key locations and reinforcer rates correlated with key locations summed over components, each expressed relative to the session average reinforcer rate, gives a good account of the data. An extension of the relative law of effect for multiple schedules fails to provide a complete account of resistance to change, but both approaches are needed for a comprehensive understanding of behavioral momentum.

References

  1. J Exp Anal Behav. 1970 Mar;13(2):243-66 [PMID: 16811440]
  2. J Exp Anal Behav. 1972 Sep;18(2):287-94 [PMID: 4675809]
  3. J Exp Anal Behav. 1992 May;57(3):317-37 [PMID: 16812656]
  4. J Exp Anal Behav. 1990 Nov;54(3):163-72 [PMID: 16812621]
  5. J Exp Anal Behav. 1988 Mar;49(2):291-302 [PMID: 16812540]
  6. J Exp Anal Behav. 1987 Jul;48(1):17-33 [PMID: 16812487]
  7. J Exp Anal Behav. 1974 May;21(3):389-408 [PMID: 16811752]
  8. J Exp Anal Behav. 1983 Mar;39(2):267-74 [PMID: 16812319]
  9. J Exp Anal Behav. 1983 Jan;39(1):49-59 [PMID: 16812312]
  10. J Exp Anal Behav. 1981 May;35(3):293-310 [PMID: 16812218]
  11. J Exp Anal Behav. 1980 May;33(3):311-26 [PMID: 16812169]
  12. Psychol Rev. 1950 Jul;57(4):193-216 [PMID: 15440996]
  13. J Exp Anal Behav. 1965 Jul;8:199-202 [PMID: 14342023]
  14. J Exp Anal Behav. 1961 Jul;4:203-8 [PMID: 13741095]
  15. J Exp Anal Behav. 1991 Sep;56(2):377-93 [PMID: 1955823]
  16. J Exp Anal Behav. 1991 Sep;56(2):345-60 [PMID: 1955821]
  17. J Exp Anal Behav. 1985 Nov;44(3):377-89 [PMID: 4086975]
  18. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 1989 Jan;32(1):347-54 [PMID: 2734345]
  19. J Exp Anal Behav. 1984 Jul;42(1):37-49 [PMID: 6481299]
  20. J Exp Anal Behav. 1986 Nov;46(3):363-79 [PMID: 3805976]
  21. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 1990 Feb;35(2):443-50 [PMID: 2320653]
  22. J Exp Anal Behav. 1990 May;53(3):359-79 [PMID: 2341820]
  23. J Exp Anal Behav. 1983 Sep;40(2):123-32 [PMID: 6631311]
  24. J Exp Anal Behav. 1969 Nov;12(6):875-85 [PMID: 16811416]

MeSH Term

Animals
Association Learning
Attention
Columbidae
Conditioning, Operant
Generalization, Psychological
Mental Recall
Models, Statistical
Motivation
Psychophysics
Reinforcement Schedule

Word Cloud

Created with Highcharts 10.0.0resistancechangecomponentsreinforcerschedulesratioratesmomentumrelativemultipletwo-keykeylocationsratedatasingle-keyequivalentproceduresscheduleshowndependcorrelatedmodelsummedaccountbehavioralBehavioralproductresponsesummarizedpigeonsrespondingtwo-componentinitiallinkschainedserialtwoobtainedpresencecomponentstimulibasedstimulus-reinforcercontingenciescombinesexpressedsessionaveragegivesgoodextensionlaweffectfailsprovidecompleteapproachesneededcomprehensiveunderstandingintegrativestudy

Similar Articles

Cited By