The role of intermittent food in the induction of attack in pigeons.

B C Yoburn, P S Cohen, F R Campagnoni
Author Information

Abstract

The present experiments evaluated whether transitions in reinforcer probability are necessary to induce attack in pigeons. In Experiment I, three of six pigeons exposed to response-contingent constant-probability food schedules and a photograph of a conspecific as a target exhibited sustained postreinforcement attack on the target. The postreinforcement pattern of attack developed over the course of the experiment and was accompanied by a reduction in the rate of postreinforcement key pecking and an increase in the postreinforcement pause in key pecking. These effects on key pecking resulted in unprogrammed variations in the probability of reinforcement which may have been responsible for the induction of attack. In Experiment II, the attack-inducing properties of a constant-probability response-independent food schedule were compared to a periodic food schedule matched for overall rate of food delivery and to a no-food condition. In addition to attack, the spatial location of the subjects was monitored during each interfood interval. The periodic and aperiodic food schedules generated very different patterns of spatial location. Postfood attack was induced by both food schedules, although the constant-probability schedule induced attack in fewer birds. The no-food condition was not effective in inducing attack in any birds. These experiments indicate that intermittent food schedules without reductions in reinforcer probability are sufficient to induce attack in some pigeons, although not as effective as schedules with transitions in reinforcer probability.

References

  1. J Exp Anal Behav. 1963 Jul;6(3):437-43 [PMID: 16811281]
  2. J Exp Anal Behav. 1963 Oct;6:607-16 [PMID: 14055961]
  3. J Exp Anal Behav. 1969 Sep;12(5):825-8 [PMID: 16811405]
  4. J Exp Anal Behav. 1969 Nov;12(6):861-74 [PMID: 16811415]
  5. J Exp Anal Behav. 1973 May;19(3):395-408 [PMID: 16811671]
  6. J Exp Anal Behav. 1974 May;21(3):563-9 [PMID: 16811764]
  7. J Exp Anal Behav. 1976 Jul;26(1):131-4 [PMID: 16811926]
  8. J Exp Anal Behav. 1980 Jul;34(1):1-12 [PMID: 16812174]
  9. J Exp Anal Behav. 1979 Jan;31(1):91-101 [PMID: 571003]
  10. Psychol Rev. 1978 Nov;85(6):571-81 [PMID: 734020]
  11. J Exp Anal Behav. 1976 Nov;26(3):349-60 [PMID: 1033979]
  12. J Exp Anal Behav. 1975 Jan;23(1):121-9 [PMID: 1167899]
  13. J Exp Anal Behav. 1968 Nov;11(6):813-7 [PMID: 5750925]
  14. Physiol Behav. 1971 May;6(5):577-88 [PMID: 5004684]
  15. Psychol Rev. 1974 Mar;81(2):119-45 [PMID: 4817611]
  16. Physiol Behav. 1980 Feb;24(2):411-4 [PMID: 7375560]
  17. Science. 1961 Jan 20;133(3447):195-6 [PMID: 13698026]
  18. J Exp Anal Behav. 1966 May;9(3):191-204 [PMID: 16811286]

Word Cloud

Created with Highcharts 10.0.0attackfoodschedulesprobabilitypigeonspostreinforcementreinforcerconstant-probabilitykeypeckingscheduleexperimentstransitionsinduceExperimenttargetrateinductionperiodicno-foodconditionspatiallocationinducedalthoughbirdseffectiveintermittentpresentevaluatedwhethernecessarythreesixexposedresponse-contingentphotographconspecificexhibitedsustainedpatterndevelopedcourseexperimentaccompaniedreductionincreasepauseeffectsresultedunprogrammedvariationsreinforcementmayresponsibleIIattack-inducingpropertiesresponse-independentcomparedmatchedoveralldeliveryadditionsubjectsmonitoredinterfoodintervalaperiodicgenerateddifferentpatternsPostfoodfewerinducingindicatewithoutreductionssufficientrole

Similar Articles

Cited By