Combining growth curves when a longitudinal study switches measurement tools.

Jacob J Oleson, Joseph E Cavanaugh, J Bruce Tomblin, Elizabeth Walker, Camille Dunn
Author Information
  1. Jacob J Oleson: Department of Biostatistics, The University of Iowa, Iowa City, USA jacob-oleson@uiowa.edu.
  2. Joseph E Cavanaugh: Department of Biostatistics, The University of Iowa, Iowa City, USA.
  3. J Bruce Tomblin: Department of Otolaryngology - Head and Neck Surgery, Department of Communication Sciences and Disorders, The University of Iowa, Iowa City, USA.
  4. Elizabeth Walker: Department of Otolaryngology - Head and Neck Surgery, Department of Communication Sciences and Disorders, The University of Iowa, Iowa City, USA.
  5. Camille Dunn: Department of Otolaryngology - Head and Neck Surgery, Department of Communication Sciences and Disorders, The University of Iowa, Iowa City, USA.

Abstract

When longitudinal studies are performed to investigate the growth of traits in children, the measurement tool being used to quantify the trait may need to change as the subjects' age throughout the study. Changing the measurement tool at some point in the longitudinal study makes the analysis of that growth challenging which, in turn, makes it difficult to determine what other factors influence the growth rate. We developed a Bayesian hierarchical modeling framework that relates the growth curves per individual for each of the different measurement tools and allows for covariates to influence the shapes of the curves by borrowing strength across curves. The method is motivated by and demonstrated by speech perception outcome measurements of children who were implanted with cochlear implants. Researchers are interested in assessing the impact of age at implantation and comparing the growth rates of children who are implanted under the age of two versus those implanted between the ages of two and four.

Keywords

References

  1. J Speech Hear Disord. 1962 Feb;27:62-70 [PMID: 14485785]
  2. Laryngoscope. 2004 Aug;114(8):1462-9 [PMID: 15280727]
  3. J Speech Lang Hear Res. 2005 Aug;48(4):853-67 [PMID: 16378478]
  4. Ear Hear. 2002 Dec;23(6):532-9 [PMID: 12476090]
  5. Biometrics. 1982 Dec;38(4):963-74 [PMID: 7168798]
  6. Int J Audiol. 2007 Sep;46(9):512-23 [PMID: 17828667]
  7. J Speech Lang Hear Res. 2012 Jun;55(3):754-63 [PMID: 22232405]
  8. Biometrics. 2002 Mar;58(1):121-8 [PMID: 11890306]
  9. Stat Med. 2014 Aug 15;33(18):3130-46 [PMID: 24723495]
  10. Biometrics. 1989 Mar;45(1):255-68 [PMID: 2720055]
  11. Ear Hear. 2014 Mar-Apr;35(2):148-60 [PMID: 24231628]
  12. Otol Neurotol. 2010 Oct;31(8):1248-53 [PMID: 20818292]

Grants

  1. M01 RR000059/NCRR NIH HHS
  2. P50 DC000242/NIDCD NIH HHS

MeSH Term

Adolescent
Age Factors
Bayes Theorem
Child
Child, Preschool
Cochlear Implantation
Deafness
Growth
Humans
Infant
Longitudinal Studies
Speech Perception
Young Adult

Word Cloud

Created with Highcharts 10.0.0growthmeasurementcurveslongitudinalchildrenagestudyimplantedtoolmakesinfluenceBayesianhierarchicaltoolsspeechperceptioncochleartwostudiesperformedinvestigatetraitsusedquantifytraitmayneedchangesubjects'throughoutChangingpointanalysischallengingturndifficultdeterminefactorsratedevelopedmodelingframeworkrelatesperindividualdifferentallowscovariatesshapesborrowingstrengthacrossmethodmotivateddemonstratedoutcomemeasurementsimplantsResearchersinterestedassessingimpactimplantationcomparingratesversusagesfourCombiningswitchesimplantmodelsmissingdatanonlinear

Similar Articles

Cited By (5)