The time course of visual influences in letter recognition.

Sylvain Madec, Kévin Le Goff, Stéphanie K Riès, Thierry Legou, Guillaume Rousselet, Pierre Courrieu, F-Xavier Alario, Jonathan Grainger, Arnaud Rey
Author Information
  1. Sylvain Madec: Laboratoire de Psychologie Cognitive-CNRS, Aix-Marseille Université, 3, place Victor Hugo-Case D, 13331, Marseille Cedex 03, France.
  2. Kévin Le Goff: Laboratoire de Psychologie Cognitive-CNRS, Aix-Marseille Université, 3, place Victor Hugo-Case D, 13331, Marseille Cedex 03, France.
  3. Stéphanie K Riès: Department of Psychology, Helen Wills Neuroscience Institute, University of California, Berkeley, CA, USA.
  4. Thierry Legou: Laboratoire Parole et Langage, CNRS & Aix-Marseille University, Aix-en-Provence, France.
  5. Guillaume Rousselet: Institute of Neuroscience and Psychology, College of Medical, Veterinary and Life Sciences, University of Glasgow, Glasgow, UK.
  6. Pierre Courrieu: Laboratoire de Psychologie Cognitive-CNRS, Aix-Marseille Université, 3, place Victor Hugo-Case D, 13331, Marseille Cedex 03, France.
  7. F-Xavier Alario: Laboratoire de Psychologie Cognitive-CNRS, Aix-Marseille Université, 3, place Victor Hugo-Case D, 13331, Marseille Cedex 03, France.
  8. Jonathan Grainger: Laboratoire de Psychologie Cognitive-CNRS, Aix-Marseille Université, 3, place Victor Hugo-Case D, 13331, Marseille Cedex 03, France.
  9. Arnaud Rey: Laboratoire de Psychologie Cognitive-CNRS, Aix-Marseille Université, 3, place Victor Hugo-Case D, 13331, Marseille Cedex 03, France. arnaud.rey@univ-amu.fr.

Abstract

This study builds on a specific characteristic of letters of the Roman alphabet-namely, that each letter name is associated with two visual formats, corresponding to their uppercase and lowercase versions. Participants had to read aloud the names of single letters, and event-related potentials (ERPs) for six pairs of visually dissimilar upper- and lowercase letters were recorded. Assuming that the end product of processing is the same for upper- and lowercase letters sharing the same vocal response, ERPs were compared backward, starting from the onset of articulatory responses, and the first significant divergence was observed 120 ms before response onset. Given that naming responses were produced at around 414 ms, on average, these results suggest that letter processing is influenced by visual information until 294 ms after stimulus onset. This therefore provides new empirical evidence regarding the time course and interactive nature of visual letter perception processes.

Keywords

References

  1. Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci. 2005 Sep;5(3):306-18 [PMID: 16396092]
  2. Comput Intell Neurosci. 2011;2011:831409 [PMID: 21403915]
  3. Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci. 2009 Jun;9(2):216-28 [PMID: 19403897]
  4. Neuroinformatics. 2010 Jun;8(2):135-50 [PMID: 20480401]
  5. Cogn Neuropsychol. 1998 Mar 1;15(1-2):203-238 [PMID: 28657523]
  6. Annu Rev Neurosci. 2004;27:649-77 [PMID: 15217346]
  7. Neuroimage. 2014 Apr 1;89:331-44 [PMID: 24321558]
  8. Trends Cogn Sci. 2007 Aug;11(8):333-41 [PMID: 17631409]
  9. Acta Psychol (Amst). 2011 Oct;138(2):322-8 [PMID: 21855049]
  10. PLoS One. 2013;8(3):e58197 [PMID: 23554876]
  11. Trends Cogn Sci. 2008 Oct;12(10):381-7 [PMID: 18760658]
  12. Front Psychol. 2011 Dec 07;2:365 [PMID: 22162973]
  13. Psychol Methods. 2003 Sep;8(3):254-74 [PMID: 14596490]
  14. Brain. 1999 Nov;122 ( Pt 11):2119-32 [PMID: 10545397]
  15. J Cogn Neurosci. 2012 Jul;24(7):1645-55 [PMID: 22185493]
  16. Cogn Neuropsychol. 2009 Feb;26(1):7-22 [PMID: 18608320]
  17. Psychon Bull Rev. 2006 Aug;13(4):674-81 [PMID: 17201369]
  18. J Neurosci Methods. 2007 Aug 15;164(1):177-90 [PMID: 17517438]
  19. Front Psychol. 2011 Jun 23;2:137 [PMID: 21886627]
  20. Psychon Bull Rev. 2012 Oct;19(5):955-61 [PMID: 22753046]
  21. J Cogn Neurosci. 2011 Jun;23(6):1419-36 [PMID: 20350170]
  22. J Neurosci Methods. 2004 Mar 15;134(1):9-21 [PMID: 15102499]
  23. Ann Biomed Eng. 2007 Sep;35(9):1532-8 [PMID: 17473984]
  24. J Cogn Neurosci. 2013 Nov;25(11):1975-85 [PMID: 23806176]
  25. Front Psychol. 2012 May 04;3:131 [PMID: 22586415]
  26. Trends Cogn Sci. 2014 Feb;18(2):90-8 [PMID: 24373885]
  27. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform. 2002 Apr;28(2):307-14 [PMID: 11999856]
  28. Neuroimage. 2007 Feb 15;34(4):1443-9 [PMID: 17188898]
  29. Brain Cogn. 2014 Jul;88:83-9 [PMID: 24880492]
  30. Brain. 2002 May;125(Pt 5):1125-36 [PMID: 11960901]
  31. Front Psychol. 2012 Jul 09;3:233 [PMID: 22787453]
  32. J Vis. 2013 Apr 25;13(5):null [PMID: 23620532]
  33. J Physiol. 1959 Oct;148:574-91 [PMID: 14403679]
  34. J Vis. 2008 Sep 16;8(12):3.1-18 [PMID: 18831616]
  35. Psychon Bull Rev. 2013 Feb;20(1):87-94 [PMID: 23132607]
  36. Brain Lang. 1996 Jun;53(3):390-407 [PMID: 8798335]
  37. Front Psychol. 2014 Oct 24;5:1213 [PMID: 25386153]
  38. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform. 2008 Oct;34(5):1317-26 [PMID: 18823214]

Grants

  1. F32 DC013245/NIDCD NIH HHS

MeSH Term

Adolescent
Adult
Evoked Potentials
Female
Humans
Male
Reaction Time
Reading
Task Performance and Analysis
Visual Perception
Young Adult

Word Cloud

Created with Highcharts 10.0.0letterslettervisuallowercaseprocessingonsetpotentialsERPsupper-responseresponsestimecourseperceptionstudybuildsspecificcharacteristicRomanalphabet-namelynameassociatedtwoformatscorrespondinguppercaseversionsParticipantsreadaloudnamessingleevent-relatedsixpairsvisuallydissimilarrecordedAssumingendproductsharingvocalcomparedbackwardstartingarticulatoryfirstsignificantdivergenceobserved120 msGivennamingproducedaround414 msaverageresultssuggestinfluencedinformation294 msstimulusthereforeprovidesnewempiricalevidenceregardinginteractivenatureprocessesinfluencesrecognitionEvent-relatedLetterVisual

Similar Articles

Cited By