Individual-level movement bias leads to the formation of higher-order social structure in a mobile group of baboons.

Tyler R Bonnell, Parry M Clarke, S Peter Henzi, Louise Barrett
Author Information
  1. Tyler R Bonnell: Department of Psychology, University of Lethbridge, Alberta, Canada. ORCID
  2. Parry M Clarke: Department of Psychology, University of Lethbridge, Alberta, Canada.
  3. S Peter Henzi: Department of Psychology, University of Lethbridge, Alberta, Canada.
  4. Louise Barrett: Department of Psychology, University of Lethbridge, Alberta, Canada.

Abstract

In mobile social groups, influence patterns driving group movement can vary between democratic and despotic. The arrival at any single pattern of influence is thought to be underpinned by both environmental factors and group composition. To identify the specific patterns of influence driving travel decision-making in a chacma baboon troop, we used spatially explicit data to extract patterns of individual movement bias. We scaled these estimates of individual-level bias to the level of the group by constructing an influence network and assessing its emergent structural properties. Our results indicate that there is heterogeneity in movement bias: individual animals respond consistently to particular group members, and higher-ranking animals are more likely to influence the movement of others. This heterogeneity resulted in a group-level network structure that consisted of a single core and two outer shells. Here, the presence of a core suggests that a set of highly interdependent animals drove routine group movements. These results suggest that heterogeneity at the individual level can lead to group-level influence structures, and that movement patterns in mobile social groups can add to the exploration of both how these structures develop (i.e. mechanistic aspects) and what consequences they have for individual- and group-level outcomes (i.e. functional aspects).

Keywords

Associated Data

figshare | 10.6084/m9.figshare.c.3808153
Dryad | 10.5061/dryad.kv2kh

References

  1. Curr Biol. 2008 Dec 9;18(23):1833-8 [PMID: 19026539]
  2. J Hum Evol. 2004 Feb;46(2):215-22 [PMID: 14871563]
  3. J Theor Biol. 2002 Sep 7;218(1):1-11 [PMID: 12297066]
  4. Elife. 2017 Jan 31;6:null [PMID: 28139196]
  5. Behav Processes. 2005 May 31;69(2):97-124 [PMID: 15845293]
  6. Proc Biol Sci. 2007 Sep 22;274(1623):2317-26 [PMID: 17644505]
  7. Ecol Lett. 2017 Jan;20(1):3-18 [PMID: 28000433]
  8. Science. 2015 Jun 19;348(6241):1358-61 [PMID: 26089514]
  9. Interface Focus. 2012 Dec 6;2(6):764-73 [PMID: 23173077]
  10. Sci Rep. 2016 Jun 13;6:27704 [PMID: 27292778]
  11. Commun Integr Biol. 2009;2(2):147-50 [PMID: 19513268]
  12. Trends Ecol Evol. 2016 Jul;31(7):550-562 [PMID: 27105543]
  13. Trends Ecol Evol. 2016 Jan;31(1):54-66 [PMID: 26552515]

Word Cloud

Created with Highcharts 10.0.0movementinfluencegrouppatternsmobilesocialcanindividualbiasnetworkheterogeneityanimalsgroup-levelgroupsdrivingsinglelevelresultsstructurecorestructuresieaspectsvarydemocraticdespoticarrivalpatternthoughtunderpinnedenvironmentalfactorscompositionidentifyspecifictraveldecision-makingchacmababoontroopusedspatiallyexplicitdataextractscaledestimatesindividual-levelconstructingassessingemergentstructuralpropertiesindicatebias:respondconsistentlyparticularmembershigher-rankinglikelyothersresultedconsistedtwooutershellspresencesuggestssethighlyinterdependentdroveroutinemovementssuggestleadaddexplorationdevelopmechanisticconsequencesindividual-outcomesfunctionalIndividual-levelleadsformationhigher-orderbaboonsattraction–repulsionmodelsleadershipecologycore/periphery

Similar Articles

Cited By