Infants' reasoning about samples generated by intentional versus non-intentional agents.

Elizabeth Attisano, Stephanie Denison
Author Information
  1. Elizabeth Attisano: Department of Psychology, University of Waterloo, Waterloo, Ontario, Canada. ORCID
  2. Stephanie Denison: Department of Psychology, University of Waterloo, Waterloo, Ontario, Canada. ORCID

Abstract

The current experiments investigate how infants use goal-directed action to reason about intentionally sampled outcomes in a probabilistic inference paradigm. Older infants and young children are flexible in their expectations of sampling: They expect random samples to reflect population statistics and non-random samples to reflect an agent's preferences or goals (Kushnir, Xu, & Wellman, 2010; Xu & Denison, 2009). However, more recent work shows that probabilistic inference comes online at approximately 6 months (Denison, Reed, & Xu, 2013; Kayhan, Gredebäck, & Lindskog, 2017; Ma & Xu, 2011; Wellman, Kushnir, Xu, & Brink, 2016), and thus, these sampling assumptions can be investigated at the age probabilistic reasoning first emerges. Results indicate that 6-month-old infants expect a human agent to sample in accord with their goal and do not expect the same of an unintentional agent-a mechanical claw. By 9.5 months, infants expect the mechanical claw to sample in accord with random sampling. These results suggest that infants use goals to make inferences about intentional sampling, under appropriate conditions at 6 months, and they have expectations of the kinds of samples a mechanical device should obtain by 9.5 months.

References

  1. Biro, S., & Leslie, A. M. (2007). Infants’ perception of goal‐directed actions: Development through cue‐based bootstrapping. Developmental Science, 10(3), 379–398. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2006.00544.x
  2. Buresh, J. S., & Woodward, A. L. (2007). Infants track action goals within and across agents. Cognition, 104(2), 287–314. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2006.07.001
  3. Carey, S. (2009). The origins of concepts. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
  4. Denison, S., Reed, C., & Xu, F. (2013). The emergence of probabilistic reasoning in very young infants: Evidence from 4.5‐and 6‐month‐olds. Developmental Psychology, 49(2), 243. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0028278
  5. Denison, S., Trikutam, P., & Xu, F. (2014). Probability versus representativeness in infancy: Can infants use naïve physics to adjust population base rates in probabilistic inference? Developmental Psychology, 50(8), 2009. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0037158
  6. Denison, S., & Xu, F. (2010). Integrating physical constraints in statistical inference by 11 month‐old infants. Cognitive Science, 34(5), 885–908. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1551-6709.2010.01111.x
  7. Denison, S., & Xu, F. (2012). Probabilistic inference in human infants. In J. B. Benson (Serial Ed.), F. Xu, & T. Kushnir (Eds.), Advances in child development and behavior: Rational constructivism in cognitive development (pp. 27–58). Waltham, MA: AcademicPress.
  8. Denison, S., & Xu, F. (2014). The origins of probabilistic inference in human infants. Cognition, 130(3), 335–347. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2013.12.001
  9. Hamlin, J. K., Hallinan, E. V., & Woodward, A. L. (2008). Do as I do: 7‐month‐old infants selectively reproduce others’ goals. Developmental Science, 11(4), 487–494. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2008.00694.x
  10. Kayhan, E., Gredebäck, G., & Lindskog, M. (2017). Infants Distinguish Between Two Events Based on Their Relative Likelihood. Child Development, https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12970
  11. Kushnir, T., Xu, F., & Wellman, H. M. (2010). Young children use statistical sampling to infer the preferences of other people. Psychological Science, 21(8), 1134–1140. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797610376652
  12. Lee, M. D., & Wagenmakers, E. J. (2013). Bayesian cognitive modeling: A practical course. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
  13. Luo, Y. (2011). Three‐month‐old infants attribute goals to a non‐human agent. Developmental Science, 14(2), 453–460. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2010.00995.x
  14. Luo, Y., & Baillargeon, R. (2005). Can a self‐propelled box have a goal? Psychological reasoning in 5‐month‐old infants. Psychological Science, 16(8), 601–608. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2005.01582.x
  15. Luo, Y., & Choi, Y. (2013). Infants attribute mental states to nonhuman agents. In M. D. Rutherford, & V. A. Kuhlmeier (Eds.), Social perception: Detection and interpretation of animacy, agency, and intention (pp. 259–282). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
  16. Ma, L., & Xu, F. (2011). Young children’s use of statistical sampling evidence to infer the subjectivity of preferences. Cognition, 120(3), 403–411. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2011.02.003
  17. Ma, L., & Xu, F. (2013). Preverbal infants infer intentional agents from the perception of regularity. Developmental Psychology, 49(7), 1330. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0029620
  18. Mahajan, N., & Woodward, A. L. (2009). Seven‐month‐old infants selectively reproduce the goals of animate but not inanimate agents. Infancy, 14(6), 667–679. https://doi.org/10.1080/15250000903265184
  19. Martin, A., Shelton, C. C., & Sommerville, J. A. (2017). Once a frog‐lover, always a frog‐lover?: Infants’ goal generalization is influenced by the nature of accompanying speech. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 146(6), 859. https://doi.org/10.1037/xge0000268
  20. Newport, E. L., & Aslin, R. N. (2004). Learning at a distance: Statistical learning of non‐adjacent dependencies. Cognitive Psychology, 48, 127–162. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0010-0285(03)00128-2
  21. Saxe, R., Tenenbaum, J. B., & Carey, S. (2005). Secret agents: Inferences about hidden causes by 10‐and 12‐month‐old infants. Psychological Science, 16(12), 995–1001. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2005.01649.x
  22. Saxe, R., Tzelnic, T., & Carey, S. (2007). Knowing who dunnit: Infants identify the causal agent in an unseen causal interaction. Developmental Psychology, 43(1), 149–158. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.43.1.149
  23. Schulz, L., Bonawitz, E. B., & Griffiths, T. L. (2007). Can being scared give you a tummy ache? Naive theories, ambiguous evidence and preschoolers' causal inferences. Developmental Psychology, 43(5), 1124–1139. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.43.5.1124
  24. Schulz, L. E., & Gopnik, A. (2004). Causal learning across domains. Developmental Psychology, 40, 162–176. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.40.2.162
  25. Téglás, E., Girotto, V., Gonzalez, M., & Bonatti, L. L. (2007). Intuitions of probabilities shape expectations about the future at 12 months and beyond. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 104(48), 19156–19159. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0700271104
  26. Téglás, E., Vul, E., Girotto, V., Gonzalez, M., Tenenbaum, J. B., & Bonatti, L. L. (2011). Pure reasoning in 12‐month‐old infants as probabilistic inference. Science, 332(6033), 1054–1059. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1196404
  27. Wellman, H. M., Kushnir, T., Xu, F., & Brink, K. A. (2016). Infants use statistical sampling to understand the psychological world. Infancy, 21(5), 668–676. https://doi.org/10.1111/infa.12131
  28. Woodward, A. L. (1998). Infants selectively encode the goal object of an actor's reach. Cognition, 69(1), 1–34. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0010-0277(98)00058-4
  29. Xu, F., & Denison, S. (2009). Statistical inference and sensitivity to sampling in 11‐month‐old infants. Cognition, 112(1), 97–104. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2009.04.006
  30. Xu, F., & Garcia, V. (2008). Intuitive statistics by 8‐month‐old infants. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 105(13), 5012–5015. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0704450105

MeSH Term

Analysis of Variance
Child Development
Concept Formation
Female
Goals
Humans
Infant
Male
Probability

Word Cloud

Created with Highcharts 10.0.0&infantsXuexpectsamplesprobabilisticsamplingmechanicaluseinferenceexpectationsrandomreflectgoalsKushnirWellmanDenison6 monthsreasoningsampleaccordclaw95 monthsintentionalcurrentexperimentsinvestigategoal-directedactionreasonintentionallysampledoutcomesparadigmOlderyoungchildrenflexiblesampling:populationstatisticsnon-randomagent'spreferences20102009HoweverrecentworkshowscomesonlineapproximatelyReed2013KayhanGredebäckLindskog2017Ma2011Brink2016thusassumptionscaninvestigatedagefirstemergesResultsindicate6-month-oldhumanagentgoalunintentionalagent-aresultssuggestmakeinferencesappropriateconditionskindsdeviceobtainInfants'generatedversusnon-intentionalagents

Similar Articles

Cited By