Varieties of Artifacts: Embodied, Perceptual, Cognitive, and Affective.

Richard Heersmink
Author Information
  1. Richard Heersmink: Department Department of Politics, Media & Philosophy, La Trobe University.

Abstract

The primary goal of this essay is to provide a comprehensive overview and analysis of the various relations between material artifacts and the embodied mind. A secondary goal of this essay is to identify some of the trends in the design and use of artifacts. First, based on their functional properties, I identify four categories of artifacts co-opted by the embodied mind, namely (a) embodied artifacts, (b) perceptual artifacts, (c) cognitive artifacts, and (d) affective artifacts. These categories can overlap and so some artifacts are members of more than one category. I also identify some of the techniques (or skills) we use when interacting with artifacts. Identifying these categories of artifacts and techniques allows us to map the landscape of relations between embodied minds and the artifactual world. Second, having identified categories of artifacts and techniques, this essay then outlines some of the trends in the design and use of artifacts, focusing on neuroprosthetics, brain-computer interfaces, and personalization algorithms nudging their users toward particular epistemic paths of information consumption.

Keywords

References

  1. Aagaard, J. (2021). 4E cognition and the dogma of harmony. Philosophical Psychology, 34(2), 165-181.
  2. Alfano, M., Carter, A., & Cheong, M. (2018). Technological seduction and self-radicalisation. Journal of the American Philosophical Association, 4(3), 298-322.
  3. Alfano, M., Fard, A., Carter, A., Clutton, P., & Klein, C. (2020). Technologically scaffolded atypical cognition: The case of YouTube's recommender system. Synthese. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11229-020-02724-x
  4. Andrada, G. (2020). Transparency and the phenomenology of extended cognition. Límite. Interdisciplinary Journal of Philosophy & Psychology, 15(12), 1-17.
  5. Atkin, A. (2008). Icon, index, and symbol. In P. Hogan (Ed.), The Cambridge encyclopaedia of language sciences (pp. 367-368). Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.
  6. Aristotle. (1984). Nichomachean ethics, metaphysics, physics. In J. Barnes, (Ed.), The complete works of Aristotle, Volumes I and II. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.
  7. Baber, C. (2003). Cognition and tool use: Forms of engagement in human and animal use of tools. London, England: Taylor and Francis.
  8. Baber, C. (2018). Designing smart objects to support affording situations: Exploiting affordance through an understanding of forms of engagement. Frontiers in Psychology, 9, 292. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00292
  9. Bach-y-Rita, P., & Kerel, S. (2003). Sensory substitution and the human-machine interface. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7(12), 541-546.
  10. Bell, G. M., & Gemmell, J. (2009). Total recall: How the e-memory revolution will change everything. New York: Dutton.
  11. Bergson, H. (1911). Creative evolution. Translated by Arthur Mitchell, New York: Dover.
  12. Brey, P. (2005). The epistemology and ontology of human-computer interaction. Minds and Machines, 15(3-4), 383-398.
  13. Burr, C., Cristianini, N., & Ladyman, J. (2018). An analysis of the interaction between intelligent software agents and human users. Minds and Machines, 28(4), 735-774.
  14. Carava, M., & Scorolli, C. (2020). When affective relation weighs more than the mug handle: Investigating affective affordances. Frontiers in Psychology, 11, 1928. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2020.01928
  15. Carter, A., Gordon, E., & Palermos, O. (2016). Extended emotion. Philosophical Psychology, 29(2), 198-217.
  16. Casati, R. (2017). Two, then four modes of functioning of the mind: Towards a unification of “dual” theories of reasoning and theories of cognitive artifacts. In J. Zacks & H. Taylor (Eds.), Representations in mind and world (pp. 7-23). New York: Routledge.
  17. Colombetti, G. (2020). Emoting the situated mind: A taxonomy of affective material scaffolds. JOLMA. The Journal for the Philosophy of Language, Mind and the Arts, 1(2), 215-236.
  18. Colombetti, G., & Roberts, T. (2015). Extending the extended mind: The case for extended affectivity. Philosophical Studies, 172(5), 1243-1263.
  19. Colombetti, G., & Krueger, J. (2015). Scaffoldings of the affective mind. Philosophical Psychology, 28, 1157-1176.
  20. Colombetti, G., Krueger, J., & Roberts, T. (2018). Editorial: Affectivity beyond the skin. Frontiers in Psychology, 9, 1307. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2018.01307/full
  21. Clark, A. (2003). Natural born cyborgs: Minds, technologies, and the future of human intelligence. Oxford, England: Oxford University Press.
  22. Clark, A. (2008). Supersizing the mind: Embodiment, action, and cognitive extension. Oxford, England: Oxford University Press.
  23. Clark, A., & Chalmers, D. (1998). The extended mind. Analysis, 58(1), 10-23.
  24. Clancey, W. (2008). Scientific antecedents of situated cognition. In P. Robbins & M. Aydede (Eds.), The Cambridge handbook of situated cognition (pp. 11-34). Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.
  25. Clowes, R. (2013). The cognitive integration of e-memory. Review of Philosophy & Psychology, 4(1), 107-133.
  26. Crete-Nishihata, M., Baecker, R., Massimi, M., Ptak, D., Campigotto, R., Kaufman, L. D., Brickman, A. M., Turner, G. R., Steinerman, J. R., & Black, S. E. (2012). Reconstructing the past: Personal memory technologies are not just personal and not just for. Human-Computer Interaction, 27(1-2), 92-123.
  27. De Cruz, H. (2008). An extended mind perspective on natural number representation. Philosophical Psychology, 21(4), 475-490.
  28. Donald, M. (1991). The origins of the modern mind: Three stages in the evolution of culture and cognition. Boston, MA: Harvard University Press.
  29. Downey, G. (2016). Sensory enculturation and neuroanthropology: The case of human echolocation. In J. Chiao, S. Li, R. Seligman, & R. Turner (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of cultural neuroscience (pp. 41-55). Oxford, England: Oxford University Press.
  30. Epstein, R. A., Patai, E. Z., Julian, J., & Spiers, H. (2017). The cognitive map in humans: Spatial navigation and beyond. Nature Neuroscience, 20(11), 1504-1513.
  31. Fasoli, M. (2018a). Substitutive, complementary, and constitutive cognitive artifacts: Developing an interaction-centered approach. Review of Philosophy and Psychology, 9(3), 671-687.
  32. Fasoli, M. (2018b). Super artifacts: Personal devices as intrinsically multifunctional, meta-representational artifacts with a highly variable structure. Minds & Machines, 28, 589-604.
  33. Fernandez, E. (2018). Development of visual neuroprostheses: Trends and challenges. Bioelectronic Medicine, 4(12), 1-8.
  34. Finley, J. R., Naaz, F., & Goh, F. W. (2018). Memory and technology: How we use information in the brain and the world. Dordrecht, The Netherlands: Springer.
  35. Gallagher, S. (2006). How the body shapes the mind. Oxford, England: Oxford University Press.
  36. Gallagher, S. (2008). Philosophical antecedents of situated cognition. In P. Robbins & M. Aydede (Eds.), The Cambridge handbook of situated cognition (pp. 35-51). Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.
  37. Gibson, J. (1979). The ecological approach to visual perception. Boston, MA: Houghton Mifflin.
  38. Griffiths, P., & Scarantino, A. (2009). Emotions in the wild: The situated perspective on emotions. In P. Robbins & M. Aydede (Eds.), The Cambridge handbook of situated cognition (pp. 437-453). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
  39. Haraway, D. (1991). A cyborg manifesto: Science, technology and socialist feminism in the late twentieth century. In D. Haraway (Ed.), Simians, cyborgs and women: The reinvention of nature (pp. 149-181). London, England: Free Association Books.
  40. Haugeland, J. (1991). Representational genera. In W. Ramsey, S. Stich, & D. Rumelhart (Eds.), Philosophy and connectionist theory (pp. 61-89). London, England: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
  41. Heersmink, R. (2013a). A taxonomy of cognitive artifacts: Function, information, and categories. Review of Philosophy and Psychology, 4(3), 465-481.
  42. Heersmink, R. (2013b). Embodied tools, cognitive tools and brain-computer interfaces. Neuroethics, 6(1), 207-219.
  43. Heersmink, R. (2016). The metaphysics of cognitive artefacts. Philosophical Explorations, 19(1), 78-93.
  44. Heersmink, R. (2018a). A virtue epistemology of the Internet: Search engines, intellectual virtues and education. Social Epistemology, 32(1), 1-12.
  45. Heersmink, R. (2018b). The narrative self, distributed memory, and evocative objects. Philosophical Studies, 175(8), 1829-1849.
  46. Heersmink, R. (2020a). Extended mind and artifactual autobiographical memory. Mind & Language. https://doi.org/10.1111/mila.12353
  47. Heersmink, R. (2020b). Varieties of the extended self. Consciousness & Cognition, 85, 103001.
  48. Heersmink, R., & Knight, S. (2018). Distributed learning: Educating and assessing extended minds. Philosophical Psychology, 31(6), 969-990.
  49. Heersmink, R., & Sutton, J. (2020). Cognition and the Web: Extended, transactive, or scaffolded? Erkenntnis, 85(1), 139-164.
  50. Heidegger, M. (1962). Being and time. London, England: SCM.
  51. Hernandez-Orallo, J., & Vold, K. (2019). AI extenders: The ethical and societal implications of humans cognitively extended by AI. Proceedings of the 2019 AAAI/ACM conference on AI, ethics, and society ( 507-513). https://doi.org/10.1145/3306618.3314238
  52. Herr, H. (2019). How we become cyborgs and extend human potential. [Video file]. Available from https://www.ted.com/talks/hugh_herr_how_we_ll_become_cyborgs_and_extend_human_potential?language=en#t-628298
  53. Hilpinen, R. (2011). Artifact. In E. Zalta (Ed.), Stanford encyclopedia of philosophy. https://plato.stanford.edu/entries/artifact/
  54. Hipolito, I., Clowes, R., & Gartner, K. (Eds.) (2021). The mind-technology problem: Investigating minds, selves and 21st century artefacts. Dordrecht, The Netherlands: Springer.
  55. Hodges, S., Williams, L., Berry, E., Izadi, S., Srinivasan, J., Butler, A., Smyth, G., Kapur, N., & Wood, K. (2006). SenseCam: A retrospective memory aid. In P. Dourish & A. Friday (Eds.), Ubicomp 2006: Ubiquitous computing (pp. 177-193). Berlin, Germany: Springer.
  56. Holzhaider, J. C., Hunt, G. R., & Gray, R. D. (2010). Social learning in new Caledonian crows. Learning & Behavior, 38(3), 206-219.
  57. Houkes, W., & Vermaas, P. (2010). Technical functions: On the use and design of artefacts. Dordrecht, The Netherlands: Springer.
  58. Hutchins, E. (1995). Cognition in the wild. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
  59. Hutchins, E. (1999). Cognitive artifacts. In R. Wilson & F. Keil (Eds.), The MIT encyclopaedia of the cognitive sciences (pp. 126-128). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
  60. Hutchins, E. (2010). Cognitive ecology. Topics in Cognitive Science, 2(4), 705-715.
  61. Ihde, D. (1990). Technology and the lifeworld: From garden to earth. Bloomington, Indiana: Indiana University Press.
  62. Ihde, D., & Malafouris, L. (2019). Homo faber revisited: Postphenomenology and material engagement theory. Philosophy & Technology, 32(2), 195-214.
  63. Kisely, S., Li, A., Warren, N., & Siskind, D. (2018). A systematic review and meta-analysis of deep brain stimulation for depression. Depression and Anxiety, 35, 468-480.
  64. Kirsh, D. (1995). The intelligent use of space. Artificial Intelligence, 72, 1-52.
  65. Kirsh, D., & Maglio, P. (1994). On distinguishing epistemic from pragmatic action. Cognitive Science, 18, 513-549.
  66. Kramer, A., Guillory, J., & Hancock, J. (2014). Experimental evidence of massive-scale emotional contagion through social networks. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 111(24), 8788-8790.
  67. Krueger, J., & Szanto, T. (2016). Extended emotions Extended emotions. Philosophy Compass, 11(12), 863-878.
  68. Laland, K., Odling-Smee, J., & Feldman, M. (2000). Niche construction, biological evolution, and cultural change. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 23, 131-175.
  69. Latour, B. (2005). Reassembling the social: An introduction to actor-network theory. Oxford, England: Oxford University Press.
  70. Merleau-Ponty, M. (1965). Phenomenology of perception. London, England: Routledge.
  71. Miller, B., & Record, I. (2013). Justified belief in a digital age: On the epistemic implications of secret Internet technologies. Episteme, 10(2), 117-134.
  72. Mittelstadt, B., Allo, P., Taddeo, M., Wachter, S., & Floridi, L. (2016). The ethics of algorithms: Mapping the debate. Big Data & Society, 3(2). https://doi.org/10.1177/2053951716679679
  73. Mueller, P.A., & Oppenheimer, D.M. (2014). The pen is mightier than the keyboard: Advantages of longhand over laptop note taking. Psychological Science, 25(6), 1159-1168.
  74. Newen, A., De Bruin, L., & Gallagher, S. (Eds.) (2018). The Oxford handbook of 4E cognition. Oxford, England: Oxford University Press.
  75. Nguyen, C. T. (2020). Echo chambers and epistemic bubbles. Episteme, 17(2), 141-161.
  76. Nickerson, R. (2005). Technology and cognition amplification. In R. Steinberg & D. Preiss (Eds.), Intelligence and technology: The impact of tools on the nature and development of human abilities (pp. 3-27). London, England: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
  77. Norman, D. (1991). Cognitive artifacts. In J. Carroll (Ed.), Designing interaction: Psychology at the human-computer interface (pp. 17-38). Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.
  78. Norman, D. (1993). Things that make us smart: Defending human attributes in the age of the machine. New York: Basic Books.
  79. Peirce, C. S. (1935a). The collected papers of Charles S. Peirce Vol 2. Boston, MA: Harvard University Press.
  80. Peirce, C. S. (1935b). The collected papers of Charles S. Peirce Vol 3. Boston, MA: Harvard University Press.
  81. Piredda, G. (2020). What is an affective artifact? A further development in situated affectivity. Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences, 19(3), 549-567.
  82. Piredda, G., & Di Francesco, M. (2020). Overcoming the past-endorsement criterion: Toward a transparency-based mark of the mental. Frontiers in Psychology, 11, 1278. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2020.01278
  83. Preston, B. (2013). A philosophy of material culture: Action, function, and mind. New York: Routledge.
  84. Preston, B. (2018). Artifact. In E. Zalta (Ed.), Stanford encyclopedia of philosophy. Available from https://plato.stanford.edu/entries/artifact/
  85. Rietveld, E., & Kiverstein, J. (2014). A rich landscape of affordances. Ecological Psychology, 26, 325-352.
  86. Robbins, P., & Aydede, M. (Eds.). (2008). The Cambridge handbook of situated cognition. Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.
  87. Saarinen, J. (2020). What can the concept of affective scaffolding do for us? Philosophical Psychology, 33(6), 820-839.
  88. Scarantino, A., & de Sousa, R. (2018). Emotion. In E. Zalta (Ed.), Stanford encyclopedia of philosophy. Available from https://plato.stanford.edu/entries/emotion/
  89. Schettler, A., Raja, V., & Anderson, M. (2019). The embodiment of objects: Review, analysis, and future directions. Frontiers in Neuroscience, 13, 1332. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnins.2019.01332
  90. Sennett, R. (2008). The craftsman. London, England: Penguin Books.
  91. Simpson, D. (2012). Evaluating Google as an epistemic tool. Metaphilosophy, 43(4), 426-445.
  92. Slaby, J. (2014). Emotions and the extended mind. In C. von Scheve & M. Salmella (Eds.), Collective emotions: Perspectives from psychology, philosophy, and sociology (pp. 32-46). Oxford, England: Oxford University Press.
  93. Smart, P., Clowes, R., & Heersmink, R. (2017). Minds online: The interface between Web science, cognitive science, and the philosophy of mind. Foundations and Trends in Web Science, 6(1-2), 1-234.
  94. Smart, P., Heersmink, R., & Clowes, R. (2017). The cognitive ecology of the Internet. In S. Cowley & F. Vallée-Tourangeau (Eds.), Cognition beyond the brain: Computation, interactivity and human artifice. ( 2nd ed., pp. 251-282). Dordrecht, The Netherlands: Springer.
  95. Smart, P., & Shadbolt, N. (2018). The world wide web. In J. Chase & D. Coady (Eds.), Routledge handbook of applied epistemology. New York: Routledge.
  96. Solsman, J. (2018). YouTube's AI is the puppet master over most of what you watch. Available from https://www.cnet.com/news/youtube-ces-2018-neal-mohan/
  97. Srinivasan, S., Diaz, M., Carty, M., & Herr, H. (2019). Towards functional restoration for persons with limb amputation: A dual-stage implementation of regenerative agonist-antagonist myoneural interfaces. Scientific Reports, 9, 1981.
  98. Sterelny, K. (2003). Thought in a hostile world: The evolution of human cognition. Oxford, England: Blackwell.
  99. Sternberg, R., & Preiss, D. (Eds.) (2005). Intelligence and technology: The impact of tools on the nature and development of human abilities. London, England: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
  100. Sutton, J. (2010). Exograms and interdisciplinarity: History, the extended mind, and the civilizing process. In R. Menary (Ed.), The extended mind (pp. 189-225). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
  101. Sutton, J. (2016). Scaffolding memory: Themes, taxonomies, puzzles. In C. Stone & L. Bietti (Eds.), Contextualizing human memory: An interdisciplinary approach to understanding how individuals and groups remember the past (pp. 187-205). New York: Routledge.
  102. Teunisse, W., Youssef, S., & Schmidt, M. (2019). Human enhancement through the lens of experimental and speculative neurotechnologies. Human Behavior and Emerging Technologies, 1(4), 361-372.
  103. Tribble, E., & Sutton, J. (2011). Cognitive ecology as a framework for Shakespearean studies. Shakespeare Studies, 39, 94-103.
  104. Turkle, S. (Ed.). (2007). Evocative objects: Things we think with. Cambridge: MIT Press.
  105. Turner, R. (2014). Programming languages as technical artifacts. Philosophy & Technology, 27(3), 377-397.
  106. Yadav, D., Yadav, S., & Veer, K. (2020). A comprehensive assessment of brain computer interfaces: Recent trends and challenges. Journal of Neuroscience Methods, 346, 108918.
  107. Vaccari, A. (2016). Against cognitive artifacts: Extended cognition and the problem of defining ‘artifact’. Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences, 16, 879-892.
  108. Vaesen, K. (2012). The cognitive bases of human tool use. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 35, 203-218.
  109. Van Dijck, J. (2013). ‘You have one identity’: Performing the self on Facebook and Linked In. Media, Culture & Society, 35(2), 199-215.
  110. Viola, M. (2021). Three varieties of affective artifacts: Feeling, evaluative and motivational artifacts. Phenomenology and Mind.
  111. Wheeler, M. (2019). The reappearing tool: Transparency, smart technology, and the extended mind. AI & Society, 34(4), 857-866).
  112. Zanker, M., Rook, L., & Jannach, D. (2019). Measuring the impact of online personalisation: Past, present, and future. International Journal of Human-Computer Studies, 131, 160-168.

MeSH Term

Algorithms
Artifacts
Cognition
Humans

Word Cloud

Created with Highcharts 10.0.0artifactsembodiedcategoriesartifactessayidentifyusetechniquesgoalrelationsmindtrendsdesignEmbodiedPerceptualCognitiveAffectiveprimaryprovidecomprehensiveoverviewanalysisvariousmaterialsecondaryFirstbasedfunctionalpropertiesfourco-optednamelybperceptualccognitivedaffectivecanoverlapmembersonecategoryalsoskillsinteractingIdentifyingallowsusmaplandscapemindsartifactualworldSecondidentifiedoutlinesfocusingneuroprostheticsbrain-computerinterfacespersonalizationalgorithmsnudginguserstowardparticularepistemicpathsinformationconsumptionVarietiesArtifacts:AffectAlgorithmsArtificialintelligenceCognitionEmbodimentInteractionPerceptionSituatedcognition

Similar Articles

Cited By