Naming of Stimuli in Equivalence Class Formation in Children.

Guro Granerud, Erik Arntzen
Author Information
  1. Guro Granerud: Department of Behavioral Science, Oslo Metropolitan University, Oslo, Norway.
  2. Erik Arntzen: Department of Behavioral Science, Oslo Metropolitan University, Oslo, Norway. ORCID

Abstract

In the present study, two typically developing 4-year-old children, Pete and Joe, were trained six conditional discriminations and tested for the formation of three 3-member equivalence classes. Pete and Joe did not establish the AC relation within 600 trials and were given two conditions of preliminary training, including naming of stimuli with two different stimulus sets. Pete started with preliminary training with common naming of stimuli, followed by conditional-discrimination training and testing for emergent relations, and continued with preliminary training on individual naming of stimuli, followed by the same training and testing as described previously. Joe experienced the same conditions but in reversed order. Pete responded in accordance with equivalence in the second round in the condition with common naming. In the first round of testing in the condition with individual naming, he responded in accordance with equivalence. In the condition with individual naming, Joe did not respond in accordance with stimulus equivalence but established all of the directly trained relations during training. In the condition with common naming, he responded in accordance with equivalence in the first round of testing. The results from the experiment support earlier findings that both common and individual naming could facilitate the emergence of equivalence classes.

Keywords

References

  1. J Exp Anal Behav. 1986 Nov;46(3):243-57 [PMID: 3805973]
  2. J Exp Anal Behav. 2006 Mar;85(2):247-73 [PMID: 16673828]
  3. J Exp Anal Behav. 2000 Mar;73(2):177-93 [PMID: 10784008]
  4. J Exp Anal Behav. 2016 May;105(3):409-26 [PMID: 27151740]
  5. J Exp Anal Behav. 2015 Nov;104(3):223-40 [PMID: 26676181]
  6. J Exp Anal Behav. 1984 May;41(3):251-66 [PMID: 6736856]
  7. Anal Verbal Behav. 2016 Oct 25;32(2):125-138 [PMID: 30800621]
  8. J Exp Anal Behav. 2015 Mar;103(2):332-48 [PMID: 25649198]
  9. J Exp Anal Behav. 1989 May;51(3):379-84 [PMID: 2498454]
  10. J Exp Anal Behav. 2000 Jul;74(1):101-14 [PMID: 10966098]
  11. J Exp Anal Behav. 1992 Jul;58(1):123-33 [PMID: 1645097]
  12. Am J Ment Defic. 1973 Mar;77(5):556-66 [PMID: 4122227]
  13. J Exp Anal Behav. 2015 May;103(3):524-41 [PMID: 25969335]
  14. Behav Processes. 2005 Jun 30;69(3):281-93 [PMID: 15896527]
  15. J Exp Anal Behav. 2004 May;81(3):267-88 [PMID: 15357510]
  16. Anal Verbal Behav. 1990;8:19-30 [PMID: 22477601]
  17. J Exp Child Psychol. 1993 Oct;56(2):201-39 [PMID: 8245768]
  18. J Exp Anal Behav. 2005 Jan;83(1):47-65 [PMID: 15762380]
  19. J Exp Anal Behav. 2017 Sep;108(2):269-289 [PMID: 28850667]
  20. J Exp Anal Behav. 2007 May;87(3):367-81 [PMID: 17575902]

Word Cloud

Created with Highcharts 10.0.0namingequivalencetrainingPeteJoecommontestingindividualaccordanceconditiontwopreliminarystimulirespondedroundtrainedclassesconditionsstimulusfollowedrelationsfirstNamingChildrenpresentstudytypicallydeveloping4-year-oldchildrensixconditionaldiscriminationstestedformationthree3-memberestablishACrelationwithin600trialsgivenincludingdifferentsetsstartedconditional-discriminationemergentcontinueddescribedpreviouslyexperiencedreversedordersecondrespondestablisheddirectlyresultsexperimentsupportearlierfindingsfacilitateemergenceStimuliEquivalenceClassFormationCommonIndividualStimulus

Similar Articles

Cited By