Desert Ants Learn to Avoid Pitfall Traps While Foraging.

Adi Bar, Chen Marom, Nikol Zorin, Tomer Gilad, Aziz Subach, Susanne Foitzik, Inon Scharf
Author Information
  1. Adi Bar: School of Zoology, The George S. Wise Faculty of Life Sciences, Tel Aviv University, Tel Aviv 6997801, Israel.
  2. Chen Marom: School of Zoology, The George S. Wise Faculty of Life Sciences, Tel Aviv University, Tel Aviv 6997801, Israel.
  3. Nikol Zorin: School of Zoology, The George S. Wise Faculty of Life Sciences, Tel Aviv University, Tel Aviv 6997801, Israel.
  4. Tomer Gilad: School of Zoology, The George S. Wise Faculty of Life Sciences, Tel Aviv University, Tel Aviv 6997801, Israel.
  5. Aziz Subach: School of Zoology, The George S. Wise Faculty of Life Sciences, Tel Aviv University, Tel Aviv 6997801, Israel. ORCID
  6. Susanne Foitzik: Institute of Organismic and Molecular Evolution, Johannes Gutenberg University Mainz, 55128 Mainz, Germany. ORCID
  7. Inon Scharf: School of Zoology, The George S. Wise Faculty of Life Sciences, Tel Aviv University, Tel Aviv 6997801, Israel.

Abstract

Central-place foragers, such as social insects or nesting birds, repeatedly use the same routes from and to their nests when foraging for food. Such species forage more efficiently after accumulating experience. We examined, here, a relatively neglected aspect of such an improvement with experience-the avoidance of pitfall traps. Similar pits are built by antlions, which co-occur with the ants, but they also resemble other natural obstacles. We used the desert ant , common in sandy habitats, and allowed it to forage for three successive runs for a food reward. Ant workers discovered food more slowly and in smaller numbers when pits were in their path. Pit presence also led to longer tracks by ants and slower movement. However, with experience, the ants fell into such pits less often and reached the food more quickly. To understand how past conditions affect current behavior, we investigated whether removing or adding pits led to a different result to that with a constant number of pits. Workers adjusted their behavior immediately when conditions changed. The only carryover effect was the longer tracks crossed by workers after pit removal, possibly resulting from the mismatch between the past and current conditions. Finally, the workers were more likely to fall into pits that were closer to the nest than those that were further away. This is a good example of the advantage that ambush predators can derive from ambushing their prey in specific locations.

Keywords

References

  1. PLoS One. 2012;7(3):e33506 [PMID: 22438939]
  2. Curr Zool. 2018 Oct;64(5):653-661 [PMID: 30323844]
  3. PLoS One. 2009 Aug 12;4(8):e6573 [PMID: 19672292]
  4. J Exp Biol. 2016 Aug 15;219(Pt 16):2426-9 [PMID: 27307487]
  5. Oecologia. 2010 Dec;164(4):1061-73 [PMID: 20589395]
  6. Learn Behav. 2022 Mar;50(1):71-81 [PMID: 34918204]
  7. Proc Biol Sci. 2022 Mar 9;289(1970):20212261 [PMID: 35232237]
  8. Anim Cogn. 2006 Apr;9(2):141-50 [PMID: 16374626]
  9. PLoS One. 2010 May 26;5(5):e10824 [PMID: 20520811]
  10. Am Nat. 2020 Apr;195(4):649-663 [PMID: 32216660]
  11. Behav Processes. 2019 Sep;166:103893 [PMID: 31252072]
  12. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2010 Jun 22;107(25):11638-43 [PMID: 20534539]
  13. Proc Biol Sci. 2011 Mar 22;278(1707):923-9 [PMID: 21177679]
  14. J Chem Ecol. 2004 Mar;30(3):607-17 [PMID: 15139311]
  15. Oecologia. 2004 Sep;141(1):171-8 [PMID: 15300485]
  16. Biology (Basel). 2021 Oct 22;10(11): [PMID: 34827072]
  17. Behav Ecol Sociobiol. 2009 Oct;63(12):1821-1827 [PMID: 19779627]
  18. Integr Zool. 2022 May 2;: [PMID: 35500247]
  19. Sci Adv. 2017 Apr 12;3(4):e1602187 [PMID: 28439543]
  20. Anim Cogn. 2012 Sep;15(5):797-806 [PMID: 22570181]
  21. Front Zool. 2011 Aug 27;8:21 [PMID: 21871114]
  22. J Exp Biol. 2000 Mar;203(Pt 5):857-68 [PMID: 10667968]
  23. Integr Zool. 2021 Dec 27;: [PMID: 34958517]
  24. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2006 Aug 15;103(33):12575-9 [PMID: 16888039]
  25. PLoS One. 2017 Aug 24;12(8):e0183753 [PMID: 28837675]
  26. Bioessays. 2015 Jun;37(6):701-9 [PMID: 25767065]
  27. Learn Behav. 2022 Feb 7;: [PMID: 35129828]
  28. Curr Biol. 2020 May 18;30(10):1927-1933.e2 [PMID: 32275874]
  29. PLoS One. 2020 Feb 28;15(2):e0229709 [PMID: 32109253]
  30. Oecologia. 1982 Jun;53(3):406-411 [PMID: 28311749]

Grants

  1. FO 298/31-1/Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft

Word Cloud

Created with Highcharts 10.0.0pitsfoodantsworkersconditionsforageexperiencepitfalltrapsantlionsalsodesertledlongertracksmovementpastcurrentbehaviorCentral-placeforagerssocialinsectsnestingbirdsrepeatedlyuseroutesnestsforagingspeciesefficientlyaccumulatingexaminedrelativelyneglectedaspectimprovementexperience-theavoidanceSimilarbuiltco-occurresemblenaturalobstaclesusedantcommonsandyhabitatsallowedthreesuccessiverunsrewardAntdiscoveredslowlysmallernumberspathPitpresenceslowerHoweverfelllessoftenreachedquicklyunderstandaffectinvestigatedwhetherremovingaddingdifferentresultconstantnumberWorkersadjustedimmediatelychangedcarryovereffectcrossedpitremovalpossiblyresultingmismatchFinallylikelyfallclosernestawaygoodexampleadvantageambushpredatorscanderiveambushingpreyspecificlocationsDesertAntsLearnAvoidPitfallTrapsForagingshadowcompetitionspatiallearning

Similar Articles

Cited By