Guilt is effectively induced by a written auto-biographical essay but not reduced by experimental pain.

Selina Schär, Antonia Vehlen, Julia Ebneter, Nathalie Schicktanz, Dominique J F de Quervain, Lutz Wittmann, Lutz Götzmann, Martin Grosse Holtforth, Sonja Protic, Alexander Wettstein, Niklaus Egloff, Konrad Streitberger, Kyrill I M Schwegler
Author Information
  1. Selina Schär: University Hospital of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, University of Bern, Bern, Switzerland.
  2. Antonia Vehlen: Abteilung für biologische und klinische Psychologie, University of Trier, Trier, Germany.
  3. Julia Ebneter: Psychology Department, University of Bern, Bern, Switzerland.
  4. Nathalie Schicktanz: Division of Cognitive and Molecular Neuroscience, University of Basel, Basel, Switzerland.
  5. Dominique J F de Quervain: Division of Cognitive and Molecular Neuroscience, University of Basel, Basel, Switzerland.
  6. Lutz Wittmann: International Psychoanalytic University, Berlin, Germany.
  7. Lutz Götzmann: Institute of Philosophy, Psychoanalysis and Cultural Studies, Berlin, Germany.
  8. Martin Grosse Holtforth: Psychology Department, University of Bern, Bern, Switzerland.
  9. Sonja Protic: International Psychoanalytic University, Berlin, Germany.
  10. Alexander Wettstein: Department of Research and Development, University of Teacher Education Bern, Bern, Switzerland.
  11. Niklaus Egloff: Psychology Department, University of Bern, Bern, Switzerland.
  12. Konrad Streitberger: Department of Anesthesiology and Pain Medicine, Pain Center, Inselspital, Bern University Hospital, University of Bern, Bern, Switzerland.
  13. Kyrill I M Schwegler: Division of Cognitive and Molecular Neuroscience, University of Basel, Basel, Switzerland.

Abstract

Introduction: The aim of the present study was (1) to validate the method of guilt-induction by means of a written auto-biographical essay and (2) to test whether experimental pain is apt to alleviate the mental burden of guilt, a concept receiving support from both empirical research and clinical observation.
Methods: Three independent groups of healthy male participants were recruited. Group allocation was not randomized but within group pain/sham administration was counterbalanced over the two test-days. Groups were tested in the following consecutive order: Group A: guilt induction, heat-pain/sham, N = 59; Group B: guilt induction, cold-pressure-pain/sham, = 43; Group C: emotionally neutral induction, heat-pain/sham, = 39. Guilt was induced on both test-days in group A and B before pain/sham administration. Visual analog scale (VAS) guilt ratings immediately after pain/sham stimulation served as the primary outcome. In a control group C the identical heat-pain experiment was performed like in group A but a neutral emotional state was induced.
Results: A consistently strong overall effect of guilt-induction (heat-pain: < 0.001, = 0.71; CPT-pain < 0.001, = 0.67) was found when compared to the control-condition ( = 0.25, = 0.08). As expected, heat- and cold-pressure-stimuli were highly painful in all groups ( < 0.0001, = 0.89). However, previous research supporting the hypothesis that pain is apt to reduce guilt was not replicated.
Conclusion: Although guilt-induction was highly effective on both test-days no impact of pain on behavioral guilt-ratings in healthy individuals could be identified. Guilt induction per se did not depend on the order of testing. The result questions previous experimental work on the impact of pain on moral emotions.

Keywords

References

  1. Nervenarzt. 2007 Jun;78(6):651-6 [PMID: 16832698]
  2. Psychosom Med. 1989 Jan-Feb;51(1):87-101 [PMID: 2648449]
  3. Cogn Emot. 2012;26(7):1189-207 [PMID: 22394129]
  4. Annu Rev Psychol. 2007;58:345-72 [PMID: 16953797]
  5. Can J Psychiatry. 2002 Dec;47(10):930-7 [PMID: 12553128]
  6. Behav Res Ther. 1998 Feb;36(2):195-204 [PMID: 9613025]
  7. Encephale. 2008 Jun;34(3):233-9 [PMID: 18558143]
  8. Psychiatry Res. 2007 Apr 15;150(3):313-25 [PMID: 17320971]
  9. Clin J Pain. 1997 Jun;13(2):116-37 [PMID: 9186019]
  10. J Clin Psychol. 1980 Jan;36(1):215-26 [PMID: 7391236]
  11. Psychother Psychosom Med Psychol. 2010 Nov;60(11):442-50 [PMID: 20200804]
  12. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1972 Oct;24(1):42-5 [PMID: 5079553]
  13. Schmerz. 2016 Dec;30(6):526-536 [PMID: 27324753]
  14. J Clin Psychiatry. 2004;65 Suppl 12:3-4 [PMID: 15315470]
  15. PLoS One. 2014 Apr 28;9(4):e95837 [PMID: 24776697]
  16. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2012 Dec;13(12):859-66 [PMID: 23165262]
  17. Brain. 2016 Jul;139(Pt 7):1958-70 [PMID: 27190016]
  18. Pain. 1998 Feb;74(2-3):181-7 [PMID: 9520232]
  19. Eur J Neurosci. 2008 May;27(9):2219-28 [PMID: 18430033]
  20. J Pers Soc Psychol. 2011 Mar;100(3):462-73 [PMID: 21244173]
  21. Cortex. 2014 Aug;57:60-73 [PMID: 24796219]
  22. Pain. 1999 Jun;81(3):225-235 [PMID: 10431710]
  23. J Trauma Stress. 2019 Aug;32(4):484-495 [PMID: 31291483]
  24. J Nerv Ment Dis. 2007 Oct;195(10):812-8 [PMID: 18043521]
  25. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2013 Jul;14(7):502-11 [PMID: 23719569]
  26. Psychoanal Rev. 1971 Fall;58(3):419-38 [PMID: 5150685]
  27. Psychosom Med. 2014 Jan;76(1):2-11 [PMID: 24336429]
  28. Psychosom Med. 1962 Jan-Feb;24:37-48 [PMID: 13890218]
  29. Pain. 2001 Feb 1;90(1-2):163-72 [PMID: 11166983]
  30. Psychol Rep. 1999 Aug;85(1):271-81 [PMID: 10575992]
  31. J Anxiety Disord. 2021 Aug;82:102443 [PMID: 34265540]
  32. Front Psychol. 2016 Feb 17;7:180 [PMID: 26925007]
  33. Psychosom Med. 1959 Jan-Feb;21(1):50-5 [PMID: 13634302]
  34. Gen Hosp Psychiatry. 2014 May-Jun;36(3):284-90 [PMID: 24650586]
  35. Curr Pain Headache Rep. 2011 Oct;15(5):400-6 [PMID: 21556710]
  36. Emotion. 2013 Feb;13(1):14-8 [PMID: 22985340]
  37. PLoS One. 2020 Dec 30;15(12):e0244429 [PMID: 33378345]
  38. Brain Behav. 2019 Jan;9(1):e01141 [PMID: 30506879]
  39. Pain. 2006 Aug;123(3):231-243 [PMID: 16697110]
  40. Brain. 2013 Sep;136(Pt 9):2751-68 [PMID: 23983029]
  41. Brain. 2013 Mar;136(Pt 3):815-27 [PMID: 23436504]
  42. Psychol Assess. 2006 Sep;18(3):262-8 [PMID: 16953729]
  43. Psychol Sci. 2011 Mar;22(3):334-5 [PMID: 21245493]
  44. ANS Adv Nurs Sci. 2015 Jan-Mar;38(1):20-33 [PMID: 25635503]
  45. Science. 2006 Sep 8;313(5792):1451-2 [PMID: 16960010]
  46. Transl Res. 2021 Dec;238:76-89 [PMID: 34182187]
  47. Emotion. 2009 Feb;9(1):118-22 [PMID: 19186924]
  48. J Dent Res. 2016 Jun;95(6):605-12 [PMID: 26965423]
  49. Trauma Violence Abuse. 2006 Apr;7(2):122-41 [PMID: 16534148]
  50. Scand J Pain. 2019 Apr 24;19(2):337-343 [PMID: 30422805]
  51. Am J Med. 1959 Jun;26(6):899-918 [PMID: 13649716]

Word Cloud

Created with Highcharts 10.0.0=0painguiltGroupgroupinductionguilt-inductionexperimentalpain/shamtest-daysGuiltinduced<writtenauto-biographicalessayaptresearchgroupshealthyadministrationheat-pain/shamneutralemotional001highlypreviousimpactmoralemotionsIntroduction:aimpresentstudy1validatemethodmeans2testwhetheralleviatementalburdenconceptreceivingsupportempiricalclinicalobservationMethods:ThreeindependentmaleparticipantsrecruitedallocationrandomizedwithincounterbalancedtwoGroupstestedfollowingconsecutiveorder:A:N59B:cold-pressure-pain/sham43C:emotionally39BVisualanalogscaleVASratingsimmediatelystimulationservedprimaryoutcomecontrolCidenticalheat-painexperimentperformedlikestateResults:consistentlystrongoveralleffectheat-pain:71CPT-pain67foundcomparedcontrol-condition2508expectedheat-cold-pressure-stimulipainful000189HoweversupportinghypothesisreducereplicatedConclusion:Althougheffectivebehavioralguilt-ratingsindividualsidentifiedpersedependordertestingresultquestionsworkeffectivelyreducedchronicmemorypain-pronenessstresstrauma

Similar Articles

Cited By