The typicality effect in basic needs.

Thomas Pölzler, Ivar R Hannikainen
Author Information
  1. Thomas Pölzler: Department of Philosophy, University of Graz, Attemsgasse 25/II, 8010 Graz, Austria. ORCID
  2. Ivar R Hannikainen: Department of Philosophy I, University of Granada, Campus de la Cartuja, 18011 Granada, Spain. ORCID

Abstract

According to the so-called Classical Theory, concepts are mentally represented by individually necessary and jointly sufficient application conditions. One of the principal empirical objections against this view stems from evidence that people judge some instances of a concept to be more than others. In this paper we present and discuss four empirical studies that investigate the extent to which this 'typicality effect' holds for the concept of . Through multiple operationalizations of typicality, our studies yielded evidence for a strong effect of this kind: (1) Participants tended to recall the same core examples of the concept in a free-listing task. (2) They judged some basic needs to be more typical than others. (3) The items that were judged to be more typical were listed more frequently in the free-listing task. (4) These items were listed earlier on in the free-listing task. (5) Typical basic needs, as well as non needs, were classified faster than atypical basic needs in a reaction time study. These findings suggest that the concept of basic needs may have a non-classical (e.g., exemplar or prototype) structure. If so, the quest for a simple and robust intensional analysis of the concept may be futile.
Supplementary Information: The online version contains supplementary material available at 10.1007/s11229-022-03859-9.

Keywords

References

  1. Cognition. 1988 Mar;28(1-2):73-193 [PMID: 2450717]
  2. Behav Brain Sci. 2010 Jun;33(2-3):61-83; discussion 83-135 [PMID: 20550733]
  3. J Abnorm Psychol. 1980 Apr;89(2):181-93 [PMID: 7365130]
  4. PeerJ. 2020 Jul 20;8:e9414 [PMID: 33005482]
  5. Mem Cognit. 1981 Mar;9(2):149-56 [PMID: 7242328]
  6. Front Psychol. 2016 Feb 17;7:184 [PMID: 26925011]
  7. Am Psychol. 2000 Jan;55(1):68-78 [PMID: 11392867]
  8. Philos Compass. 2021 May;16(5):e12732 [PMID: 35860457]
  9. Conscious Cogn. 2021 Jan;87:103054 [PMID: 33254053]

Word Cloud

Created with Highcharts 10.0.0needsconceptbasiceffectfree-listingtaskempiricalevidenceothersstudiestypicalityjudgedtypicalitemslistedmayanalysisAccordingso-calledClassicalTheoryconceptsmentallyrepresentedindividuallynecessaryjointlysufficientapplicationconditionsOneprincipalobjectionsviewstemspeoplejudgeinstancespaperpresentdiscussfourinvestigateextent'typicalityeffect'holdsmultipleoperationalizationsyieldedstrongkind:1Participantstendedrecallcoreexamples23frequently4earlier5Typicalwellnonclassifiedfasteratypicalreactiontimestudyfindingssuggestnon-classicalegexemplarprototypestructurequestsimplerobustintensionalfutileSupplementaryInformation:onlineversioncontainssupplementarymaterialavailable101007/s11229-022-03859-9BasicConceptualExperimentalphilosophyPrototypetheoryTypicality

Similar Articles

Cited By