Corrupt third parties undermine trust and prosocial behaviour between people.

Giuliana Spadaro, Catherine Molho, Jan-Willem Van Prooijen, Angelo Romano, Cristina O Mosso, Paul A M Van Lange
Author Information
  1. Giuliana Spadaro: Department of Experimental and Applied Psychology, Vrije Universiteit Amsterdam, Amsterdam, the Netherlands. g.spadaro@vu.nl. ORCID
  2. Catherine Molho: Institute for Advanced Study in Toulouse, Toulouse, France. ORCID
  3. Jan-Willem Van Prooijen: Department of Experimental and Applied Psychology, Vrije Universiteit Amsterdam, Amsterdam, the Netherlands. ORCID
  4. Angelo Romano: Department of Social, Economic and Organisational Psychology, Leiden University, Leiden, the Netherlands. ORCID
  5. Cristina O Mosso: Department of Psychology, University of Turin, Turin, Italy.
  6. Paul A M Van Lange: Department of Experimental and Applied Psychology, Vrije Universiteit Amsterdam, Amsterdam, the Netherlands. ORCID

Abstract

Corruption is a pervasive phenomenon that affects the quality of institutions, undermines economic growth and exacerbates inequalities around the globe. Here we tested whether perceiving representatives of institutions as corrupt undermines trust and subsequent prosocial behaviour among strangers. We developed an experimental game paradigm modelling representatives as third-party punishers to manipulate or assess corruption and examine its relationship with trust and prosociality (trust behaviour, cooperation and generosity). In a sequential dyadic die-rolling task, the participants observed the dishonest behaviour of a target who would subsequently serve as a third-party punisher in a trust game (Study 1a, N = 540), in a prisoner's dilemma (Study 1b, N = 503) and in dictator games (Studies 2-4, N = 765, pre-registered). Across these five studies, perceiving a third party as corrupt undermined interpersonal trust and, in turn, prosocial behaviour. These findings contribute to our understanding of the critical role that representatives of institutions play in shaping cooperative relationships in modern societies.

References

  1. Harding, L. What are the Panama Papers? A guide to history’s biggest data leak. Guardian (5 April 2016).
  2. Rose-Ackerman, S. Trust, honesty and corruption: reflection on the state-building process. Arch. Eur. Sociol. 42, 526–570 (2001). [DOI: 10.1017/S0003975601001084]
  3. Köbis, N. C., Van Prooijen, J. W., Righetti, F. & Van Lange, P. A. M. Prospection in individual and interpersonal corruption dilemmas. Rev. Gen. Psychol. 20, 71–85 (2016). [DOI: 10.1037/gpr0000069]
  4. Graycar, A. & Smith, R. G. Handbook of Global Research and Practice in Corruption (Edward Elgar, 2011).
  5. Mauro, P. Corruption and growth. Q. J. Econ. 110, 681–712 (1995). [DOI: 10.2307/2946696]
  6. Rose-Ackerman, S. The economics of corruption. J. Public Econ. 4, 187–203 (1975). [DOI: 10.1016/0047-2727(75)90017-1]
  7. Weisel, O. & Shalvi, S. The collaborative roots of corruption. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 112, 10651–10656 (2015). [DOI: 10.1073/pnas.1423035112]
  8. Gross, J., Leib, M., Offerman, T. & Shalvi, S. Ethical free riding: when honest people find dishonest partners. Psychol. Sci. 29, 1956–1968 (2018). [DOI: 10.1177/0956797618796480]
  9. Gründler, K. & Potrafke, N. Corruption and economic growth: new empirical evidence. Eur. J. Polit. Econ. 60, 101810 (2019). [DOI: 10.1016/j.ejpoleco.2019.08.001]
  10. Gupta, S., Davoodi, H. & Alonso-Terme, R. Does corruption affect income inequality and poverty? Econ. Gov. 3, 23–45 (2002). [DOI: 10.1007/s101010100039]
  11. Gino, F., Ayal, S. & Ariely, D. Contagion and differentiation in unethical behavior: the effect of one bad apple on the barrel. Psychol. Sci. 20, 393–398 (2009). [DOI: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02306.x]
  12. Köbis, N. C., van Prooijen, J. W., Righetti, F. & Van Lange, P. A. M. The road to bribery and corruption: slippery slope or steep cliff? Psychol. Sci. 28, 297–306 (2017). [DOI: 10.1177/0956797616682026]
  13. Rothstein, B. & Eek, D. Political corruption and social trust: an experimental approach. Ration. Soc. 21, 81–112 (2009). [DOI: 10.1177/1043463108099349]
  14. Banerjee, R. Corruption, norm violation and decay in social capital. J. Public Econ. 137, 14–27 (2016). [DOI: 10.1016/j.jpubeco.2016.03.007]
  15. Muthukrishna, M., Francois, P., Pourahmadi, S. & Henrich, J. Corrupting cooperation and how anti-corruption strategies may backfire. Nat. Hum. Behav. 1, 0138 (2017). [DOI: 10.1038/s41562-017-0138]
  16. Baumert, A., Halmburger, A., Rothmund, T. & Schemer, C. Everyday dynamics in generalized social and political trust. J. Res. Pers. 69, 44–54 (2017). [DOI: 10.1016/j.jrp.2016.04.006]
  17. Balliet, D. & van Lange, P. A. M. Trust, punishment, and cooperation across 18 societies: a meta-analysis. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 8, 363–379 (2013). [DOI: 10.1177/1745691613488533]
  18. Ostrom, E. Governing the Commons: The Evolution of Institutions for Collective Action (Cambridge Univ. Press, 1990).
  19. Powers, S. T., van Schaik, C. P. & Lehmann, L. Cooperation in large-scale human societies—what, if anything, makes it unique, and how did it evolve? Evol. Anthropol. https://doi.org/10.1002/evan.21909 (2021).
  20. Yamagishi, T. The provision of a sanctioning system as a public good. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 51, 110–116 (1986). [DOI: 10.1037/0022-3514.51.1.110]
  21. Hruschka, D. et al. Impartial institutions, pathogen stress and the expanding social network. Hum. Nat. 25, 567–579 (2014). [DOI: 10.1007/s12110-014-9217-0]
  22. Spadaro, G., Gangl, K., Van Prooijen, J.-W., Van Lange, P. A. M. & Mosso, C. O. Enhancing feelings of security: how institutional trust promotes interpersonal trust. PLoS ONE 15, e0237934 (2020). [DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0237934]
  23. Macy, M. W. & Sato, Y. Trust, cooperation, and market formation in the U.S. and Japan. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 99, 7214–7220 (2002). [DOI: 10.1073/pnas.082097399]
  24. Dinesen, P. T. Where you come from or where you live? Examining the cultural and institutional explanation of generalized trust using migration as a natural experiment. Eur. Sociol. Rev. 29, 114–128 (2013). [DOI: 10.1093/esr/jcr044]
  25. Sønderskov, K. M. & Dinesen, P. T. Trusting the state, trusting each other? The effect of institutional trust on social trust. Polit. Behav. 38, 179–202 (2016). [DOI: 10.1007/s11109-015-9322-8]
  26. Gächter, S. & Schulz, J. F. Intrinsic honesty and the prevalence of rule violations across societies. Nature 531, 496–499 (2016). [DOI: 10.1038/nature17160]
  27. Cohn, A., Maréchal, M. A., Tannenbaum, D. & Zünd, C. L. Civic honesty around the globe. Science 365, 70–73 (2019). [DOI: 10.1126/science.aau8712]
  28. Keizer, K., Lindenberg, S. & Steg, L. The spreading of disorder. Science 322, 1681–1685 (2008). [DOI: 10.1126/science.1161405]
  29. Balliet, D. & Van Lange, P. A. M. Trust, conflict, and cooperation: a meta-analysis. Psychol. Bull. 139, 1090–1112 (2013). [DOI: 10.1037/a0030939]
  30. Fehr, E. & Gächter, S. Cooperation and punishment in public goods experiments. Am. Econ. Rev. 90, 980–994 (2000). [DOI: 10.1257/aer.90.4.980]
  31. Molho, C., Tybur, J. M., Van Lange, P. A. M. & Balliet, D. Direct and indirect punishment of norm violations in daily life. Nat. Commun. 11, 3432 (2020). [DOI: 10.1038/s41467-020-17286-2]
  32. Čábelková, I. & Hanousek, J. The power of negative thinking: corruption, perception and willingness to bribe in Ukraine. Appl. Econ. 36, 383–397 (2004). [DOI: 10.1080/00036840410001674303]
  33. Bowler, S. & Karp, J. A. Politicians, scandals, and trust in government. Polit. Behav. 26, 271–287 (2004). [DOI: 10.1023/B]
  34. Halmburger, A., Rothmund, R., Schulte, M. & Baumert, A. Psychological reactions to political scandals: effects on emotions, trust, and the need for punishment. J. Polit. Psychol. 2, 30–51 (2012).
  35. Gächter, S., Renner, E. & Sefton, M. The long-run benefits of punishment. Science 322, 1510 (2008). [DOI: 10.1126/science.1164744]
  36. Croson, R. & Gächter, S. The science of experimental economics. J. Econ. Behav. Organ. 73, 122–131 (2010). [DOI: 10.1016/j.jebo.2009.09.008]
  37. van Dijk, E. & De Dreu, C. K. W. Experimental games and social decision making. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 72, 415–438 (2021). [DOI: 10.1146/annurev-psych-081420-110718]
  38. Soraperra, I. et al. The bad consequences of teamwork. Econ. Lett. 160, 12–15 (2017). [DOI: 10.1016/j.econlet.2017.08.011]
  39. Wouda, J., Bijlstra, G., Frankenhuis, W. E. & Wigboldus, D. H. J. The collaborative roots of corruption? A replication of Weisel & Shalvi (2015). Collabra Psychol. 3, 27 (2017). [DOI: 10.1525/collabra.97]
  40. Choshen-Hillel, S., Shaw, A. & Caruso, E. M. Lying to appear honest. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 149, 1719–1745 (2020). [DOI: 10.1037/xge0000737]
  41. Gerlach, P., Teodorescu, K. & Hertwig, R. The truth about lies: a meta-analysis on dishonest behavior. Psychol. Bull. 145, 1–44 (2019). [DOI: 10.1037/bul0000174]
  42. Fehr, E. & Gächter, S. Altruistic punishment in humans. Nature 415, 137–140 (2002). [DOI: 10.1038/415137a]
  43. Fehr, E. & Fischbacher, U. Third-party punishment and social norms. Evol. Hum. Behav. 25, 63–87 (2004). [DOI: 10.1016/S1090-5138(04)00005-4]
  44. Yamagishi, T. & Yamagishi, M. Trust and commitment in the United States and Japan. Motiv. Emot. 18, 129–166 (1994). [DOI: 10.1007/BF02249397]
  45. Capraro, V. & Perc, M. Mathematical foundations of moral preferences. J. R. Soc. Interface 18, 20200880 (2021). [DOI: 10.1098/rsif.2020.0880]
  46. Berg, J., Dickhaut, J. & McCabe, K. Trust, reciprocity, and social history. Games Econ. Behav. 10, 122–142 (1995). [DOI: 10.1006/game.1995.1027]
  47. Van Lange, P. A. M. & Kuhlman, D. M. Social value orientations and impressions of partner’s honesty and intelligence: a test of the might versus morality effect. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 67, 126–141 (1994). [DOI: 10.1037/0022-3514.67.1.126]
  48. R Core Team. R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing (R Foundation for Statistical Computing, 2022); http://www.R-project.org
  49. Preacher, K. J. & Hayes, A. F. Asymptotic and resampling strategies for assessing and comparing indirect effects in multiple mediator models. in. Behav. Res. Methods 40, 879–891 (2008). [DOI: 10.3758/BRM.40.3.879]
  50. Knack, S. & Keefer, P. Does social capital have an economic payoff? Q. J. Econ. 112, 1251–1288 (1997). [DOI: 10.1162/003355300555475]
  51. Lewicki, R. & Wiethoff, C. in The Handbook of Conflict Resolution: Theory and Practice (eds Deutsch, M. & Coleman, P. T.) 104–136 (Jossey-Bass, 2000).
  52. Van Lange, P. A. M. Generalized trust: four lessons from genetics and culture. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 24, 71–76 (2015). [DOI: 10.1177/0963721414552473]
  53. Algan, Y. & Cahuc, P. Inherited trust and growth. Am. Econ. Rev. 100, 2060–2092 (2010). [DOI: 10.1257/aer.100.5.2060]
  54. La Porta, R., Lopez-de-Silanes, F., Shleifer, A. & Vishny, R. W. Trust in large organizations. Am. Econ. Rev. 87, 333–338 (1997).
  55. Van Lange, P. A. M., Joireman, J. & Milinski, M. Climate change: what psychology can offer in terms of insights and solutions. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 27, 269–274 (2018). [DOI: 10.1177/0963721417753945]
  56. van Prooijen, J. W. The Moral Punishment Instinct (Oxford Univ. Press, 2017).
  57. Stagnaro, M. N., Arechar, A. A. & Rand, D. G. From good institutions to generous citizens: top-down incentives to cooperate promote subsequent prosociality but not norm enforcement. Cognition 167, 212–254 (2017). [DOI: 10.1016/j.cognition.2017.01.017]
  58. Marcin, I., Robalo, P. & Tausch, F. Institutional endogeneity and third-party punishment in social dilemmas. J. Econ. Behav. Organ. 161, 243–264 (2019). [DOI: 10.1016/j.jebo.2019.03.007]
  59. Kerr, N. L. et al. “How many bad apples does it take to spoil the whole barrel?”: social exclusion and toleration for bad apples. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 45, 603–613 (2009). [DOI: 10.1016/j.jesp.2009.02.017]
  60. Liebrand, W. B. G., Wilke, H. A. M., Vogel, R. & Wolters, F. J. M. Value orientation and conformity: a study using three types of social dilemma games. J. Confl. Resolut. 30, 77–97 (1986). [DOI: 10.1177/0022002786030001006]
  61. Brohmer, H. et al. Inspired to lend a hand? Attempts to elicit prosocial behavior through goal contagion. Front. Psychol. 10, 545 (2019). [DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.00545]
  62. Stirrat, M. & Perrett, D. I. Valid facial cues to cooperation and trust: male facial width and trustworthiness. Psychol. Sci. 21, 349–354 (2010). [DOI: 10.1177/0956797610362647]
  63. Erdfelder, E., Faul, F., Buchner, A. & Lang, A. G. Statistical power analyses using G*Power 3.1: tests for correlation and regression analyses. Behav. Res. Methods 41, 1149–1160 (2009). [DOI: 10.3758/BRM.41.4.1149]
  64. Casler, K., Bickel, L. & Hackett, E. Separate but equal? A comparison of participants and data gathered via Amazon’s MTurk, social media, and face-to-face behavioral testing. Comput. Hum. Behav. 29, 2156–2160 (2013). [DOI: 10.1016/j.chb.2013.05.009]
  65. Paolacci, G. & Chandler, J. Inside the Turk: understanding Mechanical Turk as a participant pool. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 23, 184–188 (2014). [DOI: 10.1177/0963721414531598]
  66. Hendriks, A. SoPHIE—Software Platform for Human Interaction Experiments (v3.2.1) (University of Osnabrück, 2012).

MeSH Term

Humans
Trust
Cooperative Behavior
Altruism
Games, Experimental
Prisoner Dilemma

Word Cloud

Created with Highcharts 10.0.0trustbehaviourinstitutionsrepresentativesprosocialunderminesperceivingcorruptgamethird-partyStudythirdCorruptionpervasivephenomenonaffectsqualityeconomicgrowthexacerbatesinequalitiesaroundglobetestedwhethersubsequentamongstrangersdevelopedexperimentalparadigmmodellingpunishersmanipulateassesscorruptionexaminerelationshipprosocialitycooperationgenerositysequentialdyadicdie-rollingtaskparticipantsobserveddishonesttargetsubsequentlyservepunisher1aN = 540prisoner'sdilemma1bN = 503dictatorgamesStudies2-4N = 765pre-registeredAcrossfivestudiespartyunderminedinterpersonalturnfindingscontributeunderstandingcriticalroleplayshapingcooperativerelationshipsmodernsocietiesCorruptpartiesunderminepeople

Similar Articles

Cited By