Some neighbors are more interfering: Asymmetric priming by stroke neighbors in Chinese character recognition.

Lili Yu, Qiaoming Zhang, Meiling Ke, Yifei Han, Sachiko Kinoshita
Author Information
  1. Lili Yu: School of Psychological Sciences and Macquarie University Centre for Reading (MQCR) Macquarie University, Sydney, NSW, 2109, Australia. lili.yu@mq.edu.au. ORCID
  2. Qiaoming Zhang: School of Educational Science, Ludong University, Yantai, Shandong, China.
  3. Meiling Ke: School of Educational Science, Ludong University, Yantai, Shandong, China.
  4. Yifei Han: School of Educational Science, Ludong University, Yantai, Shandong, China.
  5. Sachiko Kinoshita: School of Psychological Sciences and Macquarie University Centre for Reading (MQCR) Macquarie University, Sydney, NSW, 2109, Australia.

Abstract

Chinese is a visually complex logographic script that consists of square-shaped characters, with each character composed of strokes. Previous masked priming studies using single-character Chinese stroke neighbors (i.e., visually similar characters differing in only one or two strokes, e.g., /) have shown facilitatory or inhibitory priming effects. We tested whether the mixed pattern of stroke neighbor priming might be an instance of asymmetry in priming that has been observed previously with Japanese kana and Latin alphabets. Specifically, a prime lacking a stroke (or line segment) that is present in the target speeds up the recognition of its stroke neighbor almost as much as the identity prime (e.g., - = -), but not the converse (e.g., - >> -). Two experiments, one using a character match task and the second using lexical decision, showed a robust asymmetry in priming by stroke neighbors. The results suggest that the early letter identification process is similar across script types, as anticipated by the Noisy Channel model, which regards the first stage of visual word recognition as a language-universal perceptual process.

Keywords

References

  1. J Exp Psychol Gen. 2010 May;139(2):299-318 [PMID: 20438253]
  2. Psychol Rev. 1988 Jan;95(1):15-48 [PMID: 3353475]
  3. Mem Cognit. 2021 May;49(4):815-825 [PMID: 33469882]
  4. Behav Res Methods Instrum Comput. 2003 Feb;35(1):116-24 [PMID: 12723786]
  5. Front Psychol. 2014 Jul 29;5:781 [PMID: 25120503]
  6. Cognition. 2021 Sep;214:104771 [PMID: 34034009]
  7. Trends Cogn Sci. 2017 Oct;21(10):721-724 [PMID: 28676283]
  8. Q J Exp Psychol (Hove). 2014;67(11):2149-71 [PMID: 24773283]
  9. J Mem Lang. 2008 Nov;59(4):434-446 [PMID: 19884961]
  10. Psychol Rev. 1982 Jan;89(1):60-94 [PMID: 7058229]
  11. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform. 2006 Jun;32(3):668-87 [PMID: 16822131]
  12. Acta Psychol (Amst). 2011 Jan;136(1):35-41 [PMID: 20970102]
  13. Psychol Rev. 2012 Jul;119(3):517-45 [PMID: 22663560]
  14. Behav Res Methods. 2018 Feb;50(1):427-449 [PMID: 28425059]
  15. J Exp Psychol Gen. 1985 Sep;114(3):285-310 [PMID: 3161978]
  16. J Cogn. 2018 Jan 12;1(1):9 [PMID: 31517183]
  17. J Exp Psychol Gen. 2021 Aug;150(8):1612-1641 [PMID: 33332143]

MeSH Term

Humans
China
Language
Pattern Recognition, Visual
Perceptual Masking
Reaction Time
Reading
Recognition, Psychology
Writing

Word Cloud

Created with Highcharts 10.0.0primingstrokeneighborsChinesee-characterusinggrecognitionvisuallyscriptcharactersstrokessimilaroneneighborasymmetryprimeprocesscomplexlogographicconsistssquare-shapedcomposedPreviousmaskedstudiessingle-characteridifferingtwo/shownfacilitatoryinhibitoryeffectstestedwhethermixedpatternmightinstanceobservedpreviouslyJapanesekanaLatinalphabetsSpecificallylackinglinesegmentpresenttargetspeedsalmostmuchidentity=converse>>TwoexperimentsmatchtasksecondlexicaldecisionshowedrobustresultssuggestearlyletteridentificationacrosstypesanticipatedNoisyChannelmodelregardsfirststagevisualwordlanguage-universalperceptualinterfering:AsymmetricCharacterfrequencyreadingMaskedStroke

Similar Articles

Cited By

No available data.