Calves are socially motivated.

Thomas Ede, Daniel M Weary, Marina A G von Keyserlingk
Author Information
  1. Thomas Ede: Animal Welfare Program, Faculty of Land and Food Systems, University of British Columbia, Vancouver, BC, Canada V6T 1Z4.
  2. Daniel M Weary: Animal Welfare Program, Faculty of Land and Food Systems, University of British Columbia, Vancouver, BC, Canada V6T 1Z4.
  3. Marina A G von Keyserlingk: Animal Welfare Program, Faculty of Land and Food Systems, University of British Columbia, Vancouver, BC, Canada V6T 1Z4.

Abstract

Most dairy calves are housed individually in the first weeks and sometimes months of their lives. Lack of social interaction can negatively impact feed intake, social skills, coping abilities, and cognitive performance, but the motivation of calves to seek companionship has seldom been investigated. In this study, 10 Holstein bull calves (; averaging 5.4 ± 2.6 d old upon entering the study) were housed individually in a central home pen with access to one pen on either side, each connected by a push gate. One side pen housed another calf of similar age and the same sex, and the second was otherwise identical in size and resources (feed and water) but without a social companion. Each time the test calf pushed open the gate to access a side pen, he would be left in it until the next feeding (approximately 0800 and 1600 h), at which time he was returned to the central home pen. After each successful pushing event, additional weight was added to the gate (initially a small amount, then incrementally higher). All calves but one pushed for the first time on d 1 of enrollment (within 9.4 ± 14.8 min of experimental start); the remaining calf pushed on d 3 of the test. Each calf was tested for 15 d and we recorded the maximum weight pushed for both side pens. Calves pushed a higher maximum weight (and pushed more frequently) for access to the pen with a social partner compared with the empty pen. We conclude that calves are socially motivated, even at a young age, and that calves can benefit from access to social contact.

References

  1. PLoS One. 2014 May 21;9(5):e98429 [PMID: 24848635]
  2. J Dairy Sci. 2016 Feb;99(2):1406-1417 [PMID: 26686722]
  3. Sci Rep. 2019 Nov 6;9(1):16159 [PMID: 31695053]
  4. Biol Lett. 2018 Aug;14(8): [PMID: 30089661]
  5. J Dairy Sci. 2016 Apr;99(4):2453-2467 [PMID: 26874423]
  6. Sci Rep. 2020 Aug 28;10(1):14233 [PMID: 32859980]
  7. J Dairy Sci. 2014;97(8):5035-44 [PMID: 24856985]
  8. Behav Processes. 2010 Jul;84(3):726-31 [PMID: 20457231]
  9. PLoS One. 2017 Jan 4;12(1):e0166926 [PMID: 28052122]
  10. PLoS One. 2019 Oct 30;14(10):e0223746 [PMID: 31665176]
  11. J Dairy Sci. 2020 Sep;103(9):8507-8517 [PMID: 32622600]
  12. Animal. 2010 Sep;4(9):1588-96 [PMID: 22444707]
  13. PLoS One. 2018 Mar 7;13(3):e0193109 [PMID: 29513705]
  14. Sci Rep. 2017 Mar 23;7:44953 [PMID: 28332567]
  15. Sci Rep. 2019 Dec 27;9(1):20246 [PMID: 31882927]

Word Cloud

Created with Highcharts 10.0.0pencalvespushedsocialdaccesssidecalfhousedgatetimeweightindividuallyfirstcanfeedstudy4±centralhomeoneagetesthighermaximumCalvessociallymotivateddairyweekssometimesmonthslivesLackinteractionnegativelyimpactintakeskillscopingabilitiescognitiveperformancemotivationseekcompanionshipseldominvestigated10Holsteinbullaveraging526olduponenteringeitherconnectedpushOneanothersimilarsexsecondotherwiseidenticalsizeresourceswaterwithoutcompanionopenleftnextfeedingapproximately08001600hreturnedsuccessfulpushingeventadditionaladdedinitiallysmallamountincrementally1enrollmentwithin9148minexperimentalstartremaining3tested15recordedpensfrequentlypartnercomparedemptyconcludeevenyoungbenefitcontact

Similar Articles

Cited By