Inhibition and paradoxical choice.

Valeria V González, Aaron P Blaisdell
Author Information
  1. Valeria V González: Department of Psychology, University of California, 1285 Franz Hall, Los Angeles, CA, 90095-1563, USA. vgonzalez@psych.ucla.edu. ORCID
  2. Aaron P Blaisdell: Department of Psychology, University of California, 1285 Franz Hall, Los Angeles, CA, 90095-1563, USA.

Abstract

The present study evaluated the role of inhibition in paradoxical choice in pigeons. In a paradoxical choice procedure, pigeons receive a choice between two alternatives. Choosing the "suboptimal" alternative is followed 20% of the time by one cue (the S+) that is always reinforced, and 80% of the time by another cue (S-) that is never reinforced. Thus, this alternative leads to an overall reinforcement rate of 20%. Choosing the "optimal" alternative, however, is followed by one of two cues (S3 or S4), each reinforced 50% of the time. Thus, this alternative leads to an overall reinforcement rate of 50%. González and Blaisdell (2021) reported that development of paradoxical choice was positively correlated to the development of inhibition to the S- (signal that no food will be delivered on that trial) post-choice stimulus. The current experiment tested the hypothesis that inhibition to a post-choice stimulus is causally related to suboptimal preference. Following acquisition of suboptimal preference, pigeons received two manipulations: in one condition one of the cues in the optimal alternative (S4) was extinguished and, in another condition, the S- cue was partially reinforced. When tested on the choice task afterward, both manipulations resulted in a decrement in suboptimal preference. This result is paradoxical given that both manipulations made the suboptimal alternative the richer option. We discuss the implications of our results, arguing that inhibition of a post-choice cue increases attraction to or value of that choice.

Keywords

References

  1. J Exp Anal Behav. 2020 Sep;114(2):233-247 [PMID: 33460139]
  2. J Exp Psychol Anim Learn Cogn. 2018 Apr;44(2):180-193 [PMID: 29683696]
  3. J Exp Psychol Anim Learn Cogn. 2019 Jul;45(3):301-310 [PMID: 31070433]
  4. Learn Mem. 2004 Sep-Oct;11(5):485-94 [PMID: 15466298]
  5. Behav Processes. 2016 Sep;130:65-70 [PMID: 27421608]
  6. Behav Processes. 2016 May;126:82-93 [PMID: 27005579]
  7. Ann Neurosci. 2010 Jul;17(3):136-41 [PMID: 25205891]
  8. Psychon Bull Rev. 2010 Jun;17(3):412-6 [PMID: 20551367]
  9. Sci Rep. 2015 Sep 09;5:13874 [PMID: 26350951]
  10. J Exp Psychol Anim Learn Cogn. 2016 Oct;42(4):336-346 [PMID: 27598063]
  11. Anim Cogn. 2019 Jan;22(1):81-87 [PMID: 30430348]
  12. Learn Behav. 2003 Feb;31(1):35-48 [PMID: 18450068]
  13. J Exp Anal Behav. 2020 May;113(3):591-608 [PMID: 32237091]
  14. J Exp Psychol Anim Learn Cogn. 2016 Jan;42(1):1-14 [PMID: 26640967]
  15. J Exp Psychol Anim Learn Cogn. 2014 Jan;40(1):12-21 [PMID: 24893105]
  16. J Exp Anal Behav. 1967 Sep;10(5):417-24 [PMID: 6050053]
  17. Psychon Bull Rev. 2022 Aug;29(4):1514-1523 [PMID: 35378670]
  18. Behav Processes. 2018 Dec;157:320-326 [PMID: 30077654]
  19. J Exp Anal Behav. 2016 Jan;105(1):23-40 [PMID: 26781050]
  20. J Exp Psychol Anim Learn Cogn. 2021 Oct;47(4):429-444 [PMID: 34855433]
  21. Emotion. 2009 Feb;9(1):123-7 [PMID: 19186925]
  22. Anim Cogn. 2023 Mar;26(2):623-637 [PMID: 36306041]

MeSH Term

Animals
Reinforcement Schedule
Choice Behavior
Reinforcement, Psychology
Inhibition, Psychological
Cues
Columbidae