Figurative language processing in autism spectrum disorders: A review.

Stella Lampri, Eleni Peristeri, Theodoros Marinis, Maria Andreou
Author Information
  1. Stella Lampri: Department of Speech and Language Therapy, University of Peloponnese, Kalamata, Greece.
  2. Eleni Peristeri: Department of Theoretical and Applied Linguistics, School of English, Aristotle University of Thessaloniki, Thessaloniki, Greece. ORCID
  3. Theodoros Marinis: Department of Linguistics, University of Konstanz, Konstanz, Germany.
  4. Maria Andreou: Department of Speech and Language Therapy, University of Peloponnese, Kalamata, Greece. ORCID

Abstract

Impairments in the broader domain of pragmatics are considered to be a defining feature of Autism Spectrum Disorders (ASD). A challenging aspect of pragmatic competence is the ability to process nonliteral language. Interestingly, previous studies in figurative language comprehension in ASD have demonstrated conflicting results regarding participants' performance. The main scientific debate focuses on the underlying skills which facilitate processing of nonliteral speech in ASD. Namely, Theory of Mind (ToM), language abilities and Executive functions (EFs) are regarded as factors affecting autistic individuals' performance. This review addresses figurative language comprehension in ASD in light of the above three interpretive accounts. We reviewed data from recent studies in this field concluding that autistic children indeed encounter systematic difficulties in the processing of non-literal language. Moreover, only ToM and verbal skills were found to correlate the most with figurative language comprehension in ASD. Notably, we found that differences related to research methodology and tasks' properties may have led to discrepancies between studies' results. Finally, we argue that future studies should encompass in their experimental design figurative comprehension tasks with minimal linguistic demands and also measures of ToM, verbal ability and EFs in order to shed more light in the independent contribution of those skills to the processing of nonliteral language in ASD.

Keywords

References

  1. Adachi, T., Koeda, T., Hirabayashi, S., Maeoka, Y., Shiota, M., Charles Wright, E., & Wada, A. (2004). The metaphor and sarcasm scenario test: A new instrument to help differentiate high functioning pervasive developmental disorder from attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Brain and Development, 26(5), 301–306. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0387-7604(03)00170-0
  2. Aguert, M., Laval, V., Gauducheau, N., Atifi, H., & Marcoccia, M. (2016). Producing irony in adolescence: A comparison between face‐to‐face and computer‐mediated communication. Psychology of Language and Communication, 20(3), 199–218. https://doi.org/10.1515/plc-2016-0013
  3. American Psychiatric Publishing, Inc. (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders: DSM‐5™ (5th ed., p. 947). American Psychiatric Association Publishing. https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.books.9780890425596
  4. Anderson, V. (1998). Assessing executive functions in children: Biological, psychological, and developmental considerations. Neuropsychological Rehabilitation, 8(3), 319–349. https://doi.org/10.1080/713755568
  5. Andreou, M., & Skrimpa, V. (2020). Theory of mind deficits and neurophysiological operations in autism Spectrum disorders: A review. Brain Sciences, 10(6), 393. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci10060393
  6. Andreou, M., & Skrimpa, V. (2022). Re‐examining labels in neurocognitive research: Evidence from bilingualism and autism as Spectrum‐trait cases. Brain Sciences, 12(8), 1113. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci12081113
  7. Andreou, M., Tsimpli, I. M., Durrleman, S., & Peristeri, E. (2020). Theory of mind, executive functions, and syntax in bilingual children with autism Spectrum disorder. Language, 5(4), 67. https://doi.org/10.3390/languages5040067
  8. Angeleri, R., & Airenti, G. (2014). The development of joke and irony understanding: A study with 3‐ to 6‐year‐old children. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, 68(2), 133–146. https://doi.org/10.1037/cep0000011
  9. Baixauli‐Fortea, I., Miranda Casas, A., Berenguer‐Forner, C., Colomer‐Diago, C., & Roselló‐Miranda, B. (2019). Pragmatic competence of children with autism spectrum disorder. Impact of theory of mind, verbal working memory, ADHD symptoms, and structural language. Applied Neuropsychology: The Child, 8(2), 101–112. https://doi.org/10.1080/21622965.2017.1392861
  10. Baron‐Cohen, S., Wheelwright, S., Spong, A., Scahill, V., & Lawson, J. (2001). Are intuitive physics and intuitive psychology independent? A Test with Children with Asperger Syndrome. Journal of Developmental and Learning Disorders, 5(1),47–78. https://www.semanticscholar.org/paper/Are‐intuitive‐physics‐and‐intuitive‐psychology‐A‐Baron‐Cohen‐Wheelwright/4e4c54bc85b05d48e780e3fe32a821fb876b6606
  11. Begeer, S., Malle, B. F., Nieuwland, M. S., & Keysar, B. (2010). Using theory of mind to represent and take part in social interactions: Comparing individuals with high‐functioning autism and typically developing controls. European Journal of Developmental Psychology, 7(1), 104–122. https://doi.org/10.1080/17405620903024263
  12. Benedek, M., Beaty, R., Jauk, E., Koschutnig, K., Fink, A., Silvia, P. J., Dunst, B., & Neubauer, A. C. (2014). Creating metaphors: The neural basis of figurative language production. NeuroImage, 90, 99–106. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.12.046
  13. Berber Sardinha, T. (2008). Metaphor probabilities in corpora. In M.S. Zanotto, L. Cameron & M.C. Cavalcanti (Eds.), Confronting Metaphor in Use: An Applied Linguistic Approach (pp.127–148). John Benjamins. https://doi.org/10.1075/pbns.173.09ber
  14. Bernicot, J., Laval, V., & Chaminaud, S. (2007). Nonliteral language forms in children: In what order are they acquired in pragmatics and metapragmatics? Journal of Pragmatics, 39(12), 2115–2132. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pragma.2007.05.009
  15. Bowdle, B. F., & Gentner, D. (2005). The career of metaphor. Psychological Review, 112(1), 193–216. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.112.1.193
  16. Brynskov, C., Krøjgaard, P., & Eigsti, I.‐M. (2016). Language and communication in children with autism: Do research and clinical practice converge in Denmark? Nordic Psychology, 68(2), 100–113. https://doi.org/10.1080/19012276.2015.1071199
  17. Burgers, C. F., & van Mulken, M. J. P. (2017). Humor Markers. In S. Attardo (Ed.), The Routledge Handbook of Language and Humor (1st ed., pp. 385–399). Routledge. https://repository.ubn.ru.nl/handle/2066/173627
  18. Cacciari, C. (2014). Processing multiword idiomatic strings: Many words in one? The Mental Lexicon, 9(2), 267–293. https://doi.org/10.1075/ml.9.2.05cac
  19. Cardillo, R., Mammarella, I. C., Demurie, E., Giofrè, D., & Roeyers, H. (2021). Pragmatic language in children and adolescents with autism Spectrum disorder: Do theory of mind and executive functions have a mediating role? Autism Research: Official Journal of the International Society for Autism Research, 14(5), 932–945. https://doi.org/10.1002/aur.2423
  20. Carston, R. (2010). Metaphor: Ad hoc concepts, literal meaning and mental images. Proceedings of the Aristotelian Society, 110, 295–321. https://www.jstor.org/stable/41061519
  21. Chahboun, S., Vulchanov, V., Saldaña, D., Eshuis, H., & Vulchanova, M. (2016). Can you play with fire and not hurt yourself? A comparative study in figurative language comprehension between individuals with and without autism Spectrum disorder. PLoS ONE, 11(12), e0168571. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0168571
  22. Chahboun, S., Vulchanov, V., Saldaña, D., Eshuis, H., & Vulchanova, M. (2017). Can you tell it by the prime? A study of metaphorical priming in high‐functioning autism in comparison with matched controls: Metaphorical priming in high‐functioning autism and matched controls. International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders, 52(6), 766–785. https://doi.org/10.1111/1460-6984.12314
  23. Chiappe, D. L., & Chiappe, P. (2007). The role of working memory in metaphor production and comprehension. Journal of Memory and Language, 56(2), 172–188. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2006.11.006
  24. Chouinard, B., & Cummine, J. (2016). All the world's a stage: Evaluation of two stages of metaphor comprehension in people with autism spectrum disorder. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 23, 107–121. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.rasd.2015.12.008
  25. Colich, N. L., Wang, A.‐T., Rudie, J. D., Hernandez, L. M., Bookheimer, S. Y., & Dapretto, M. (2012). Atypical neural processing of ironic and sincere remarks in children and adolescents with autism Spectrum disorders. Metaphor and Symbol, 27(1), 70–92. https://doi.org/10.1080/10926488.2012.638856
  26. Cummings, L. (2009). Clinical Pragmatics. Cambridge University Press.
  27. Deliens, G., Papastamou, F., Ruytenbeek, N., Geelhand, P., & Kissine, M. (2018). Selective pragmatic impairment in autism Spectrum disorder: Indirect requests versus irony. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 48(9), 2938–2952. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-018-3561-6
  28. Dews, S., & Winner, E. (1995). Muting the meaning: A social function of irony. Metaphor and Symbolic Activity, 10(1), 3–19. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327868ms1001_2
  29. Dietrich, A. (2004). The cognitive neuroscience of creativity. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 11(6), 1011–1026. https://doi.org/10.3758/bf03196731
  30. Eigsti, I.‐M. (2013). A review of embodiment in autism Spectrum disorders. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 224. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00224
  31. Falkum, I. L., Recasens, M., & Clark, E. V. (2017). “The moustache sits down first”: On the acquisition of metonymy. Journal of Child Language, 44(1), 87–119. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0305000915000720
  32. Filippova, E. (2014). Irony production and comprehension. In D. Matthews (Ed.), Pragmatic development in first language acquisition (pp. 261–278). John Benjamins Publishing Company. https://doi.org/10.1075/tilar.10.15fil
  33. Filippova, E., & Astington, J. W. (2008). Further development in social reasoning revealed in discourse irony understanding. Child Development, 79(1), 126–138. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2007.01115.x
  34. Filippova, E., & Astington, J. W. (2010). Children's understanding of social‐cognitive and social‐communicative aspects of discourse irony. Child Development, 81(3), 913–928. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2010.01442.x
  35. Gernsbacher, M. A., & Pripas‐Kapit, S. R. (2012). Who's missing the point? A commentary on claims that autistic persons have a specific deficit in figurative language comprehension. Metaphor and Symbol, 27(1), 93–105. https://doi.org/10.1080/10926488.2012.656255
  36. Geurts, B., Kissine, M., & van Tiel, B. (2019). Pragmatic reasoning in autism. In K. Morsanyi & R.M.J. Byrne (Eds.), Thinking, Reasoning and Desicion Making in Autism (1st ed.). Routledge. https://doi.org/10.4324/9781351060912-6
  37. Gibbs, R. W., Jr., & Colston, H. L. (2012). Interpreting figurative meaning (Vol. 384). Cambridge University Press. https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9781139168779
  38. Gibbs, R. W., Bryant, G. A., & Colston, H. L. (2014). Where is the humor in verbal irony? Humor, 27(4), 575–595. https://doi.org/10.1515/humor-2014-0106
  39. Giora, R. (1997). Understanding figurative and literal language: The graded salience hypothesis. Cognitive Linguistics, 8(3), 183–206. https://doi.org/10.1515/cogl.1997.8.3.183
  40. Giora, R., Gazal, O., Goldstein, I., Fein, O., & Stringaris, A. (2012). Salience and context: Interpretation of metaphorical and literal language by young adults diagnosed with Asperger's syndrome. Metaphor and Symbol, 27(1), 22–54. https://doi.org/10.1080/10926488.2012.638823
  41. Glenwright, M., & Agbayewa, A. S. (2012). Older children and adolescents with high‐functioning autism spectrum disorders can comprehend verbal irony in computer‐mediated communication. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 6(2), 628–638. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.rasd.2011.09.013
  42. Glucksberg, S. (1991). Beyond literal meanings: The psychology of allusion. Psychological Science, 2(3), 146–152. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.1991.tb00122.x
  43. Glucksberg, S. (2001). Understanding figurative language: From metaphors to idioms (pp. vii–134). Oxford University Press. https://doi.org/10.1093/acprof:oso/9780195111095.001.0001
  44. Gold, R., & Faust, M. (2010). Right hemisphere dysfunction and metaphor comprehension in young adults with Asperger syndrome. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 40(7), 800–811. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-009-0930-1
  45. Gold, R., Faust, M., & Goldstein, A. (2010). Semantic integration during metaphor comprehension in Asperger syndrome. Brain and Language, 113(3), 124–134. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandl.2010.03.002
  46. Greimel, E., Nehrkorn, B., Fink, G. R., Kukolja, J., Kohls, G., Müller, K., Piefke, M., Kamp‐Becker, I., Remschmidt, H., Herpertz‐Dahlmann, B., Konrad, K., & Schulte‐Rüther, M. (2012). Neural mechanisms of encoding social and non‐social context information in autism spectrum disorder. Neuropsychologia, 50(14), 3440–3449. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2012.09.029
  47. Hancock, J. T., Dunham, P. J., & Purdy, K. (2000). Children's comprehension of critical and complimentary forms of verbal irony. Journal of Cognition and Development, 1(2), 227–248. https://doi.org/10.1207/S15327647JCD010204
  48. Happé, F. (1999). Autism: Cognitive deficit or cognitive style? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 3(6), 216–222. https://doi.org/10.1016/S1364-6613(99)01318-2
  49. Happé, F. G. (1993). Communicative competence and theory of mind in autism: A test of relevance theory. Cognition, 48(2), 101–119. https://doi.org/10.1016/0010-0277(93)90026-r
  50. Happé, F. G. E. (1994). An advanced test of theory of mind: Understanding of story characters' thoughts and feelings by able autistic, mentally handicapped, and normal children and adults. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 24(2), 129–154. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF02172093
  51. Happé, F. G. E. (1995). Understanding minds and metaphors: Insights from the study of figurative language in autism. Metaphor and Symbolic Activity, 10(4), 275–295. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327868ms1004_3
  52. Hill, E. L. (2004). Executive dysfunction in autism. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8(1), 26–32. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2003.11.003
  53. Holyoak, K. J. (2019). The Spider's thread: Metaphor in mind, brain, and poetry. The MIT Press. https://doi.org/10.7551/mitpress/11119.001.0001
  54. Hoogenhout, M., & Malcolm‐Smith, S. (2017). Theory of mind predicts severity level in autism. Autism: The International Journal of Research and Practice, 21(2), 242–252. https://doi.org/10.1177/1362361316636758
  55. Huang, S.‐F., Oi, M., & Taguchi, A. (2015). Comprehension of figurative language in Taiwanese children with autism: The role of theory of mind and receptive vocabulary. Clinical Linguistics and Phonetics, 29(8–10), 764–775. https://doi.org/10.3109/02699206.2015.1027833
  56. Iskandar, S., & Baird, A. D. (2014). The role of working memory and divided attention in metaphor interpretation. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 43(5), 555–568. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10936-013-9267-1
  57. Järvinen‐Pasley, A., Pasley, J., & Heaton, P. (2008). Is the linguistic content of speech less salient than its perceptual features in autism? Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 38(2), 239–248. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-007-0386-0
  58. Kalandadze, T., Bambini, V., & Næss, K.‐A. B. (2019). A systematic review and meta‐analysis of studies on metaphor comprehension in individuals with autism spectrum disorder: Do task properties matter? Applied PsychoLinguistics, 40(6), 1421–1454. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0142716419000328
  59. Kalandadze, T., Braeken, J., Brynskov, C., & Næss, K.‐A. B. (2022). Metaphor comprehension in individuals with autism Spectrum disorder: Core language skills matter. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 52(1), 316–326. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-021-04922-z
  60. Kalandadze, T., Norbury, C., Nærland, T., & Næss, K.‐A. B. (2018). Figurative language comprehension in individuals with autism spectrum disorder: A meta‐analytic review. Autism: The International Journal of Research and Practice, 22(2), 99–117. https://doi.org/10.1177/1362361316668652
  61. Kasirer, A., & Mashal, N. (2014). Verbal creativity in autism: Comprehension and generation of metaphoric language in high‐functioning autism spectrum disorder and typical development. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 8, 615. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2014.00615
  62. Kasirer, A., & Mashal, N. (2016). Comprehension and generation of metaphors by children with autism spectrum disorder. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 32, 53–63. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.rasd.2016.08.003
  63. Korkman, M., Kirk, U., & Kemp, S. (2007). NEPSY II: Clinical and interpretive manual. Harcourt Assessment, PsychCorp.
  64. Kouklari, E.‐C., Tsermentseli, S., & Monks, C. P. (2018). Hot and cool executive function in children and adolescents with autism spectrum disorder: Cross‐sectional developmental trajectories. Child Neuropsychology: A Journal on Normal and Abnormal Development in Childhood and Adolescence, 24(8), 1088–1114. https://doi.org/10.1080/09297049.2017.1391190
  65. Lada, A., Paquier, P., Manouilidou, C., & Keulen, S. (2023). A systematic review: Idiom comprehension in aphasia: The effects of stimuli and task type. Journal of Neurolinguistics, 65, 101115. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jneuroling.2022.101115
  66. Lai, C. L. E., Lau, Z., Lui, S. S. Y., Lok, E., Tam, V., Chan, Q., Cheng, K. M., Lam, S. M., & Cheung, E. F. C. (2017). Meta‐analysis of neuropsychological measures of executive functioning in children and adolescents with high‐functioning autism spectrum disorder. Autism Research: Official Journal of the International Society for Autism Research, 10(5), 911–939. https://doi.org/10.1002/aur.1723
  67. Landa, R. (2000). Social language use in Asperger syndrome and high‐functioning autism. In A. Klin, F. R. Volkmar & S. S. Sparrow (Eds.), Asperger syndrome (Vol. 18, pp. 125–155). The Guilford Press. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00787-008-0701-0
  68. Landa, R. J., & Goldberg, M. C. (2005). Language, social, and executive functions in high functioning autism: A continuum of performance. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 35(5), 557–573. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-005-0001-1
  69. Le Sourn‐Bissaoui, S., Caillies, S., Gierski, F., & Motte, J. (2009). Inference processing in adolescents with Asperger syndrome: Relationship with theory of mind abilities. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 3(3), 797–808. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.rasd.2009.03.003
  70. Levorato, M. C. (1999). Idiom comprehension in children: Are the effects of semantic Analysability and context separable? European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 11(1), 51–66. https://doi.org/10.1080/713752299
  71. Liu, M.‐J., Shih, W.‐L., & Ma, L.‐Y. (2011). Are children with Asperger syndrome creative in divergent thinking and feeling? A brief report. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 5(1), 294–298. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.rasd.2010.04.011
  72. López, B., & Leekam, S. R. (2003). Do children with autism fail to process information in context?: Use of context in autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 44(2), 285–300. https://doi.org/10.1111/1469-7610.00121
  73. Loukusa, S., & Leinonen, E. (2008). Development of comprehension of ironic utterances in 3‐ to 9‐year‐old Finnish‐speaking children. Psychology of Language and Communication, 12, 55–69. https://doi.org/10.2478/v10057-008-0003-0
  74. MacKay, G., & Shaw, A. (2004). A comparative study of figurative language in children with autistic spectrum disorders. Child Language Teaching and Therapy, 20(1), 13–32. https://doi.org/10.1191/0265659004ct261oa
  75. Marini, A., Marotta, L., Bulgheroni, S., & Fabbro, F. (2015). Batteria per la valutazione del linguaggio in bambini dai 4 ai 12 anni (BVL_4–12). Giunti O.S.
  76. Marinis, T., Andreou, M., Bagioka, D. V., Baumeister, F., Bongartz, C., Czypionka, A., Golegos, A., Peristeri, E., Skrimpa, V., Durrleman, S., & Terzi, A. (2023). Development and validation of a task battery for verbal and non‐verbal first‐ and second‐order theory of mind. Frontiers in Language Sciences, 1. https://doi.org/10.3389/flang.2022.1052095
  77. Martin, I., & McDonald, S. (2003). Weak coherence, no theory of mind, or executive dysfunction? Solving the puzzle of pragmatic language disorders. Brain and Language, 85(3), 451–466. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0093-934x(03)00070-1
  78. Mashal, N., & Kasirer, A. (2011). Thinking maps enhance metaphoric competence in children with autism and learning disabilities. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 32(6), 2045–2054. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ridd.2011.08.012
  79. Melogno, S., Pinto, M., & Di Filippo, G. (2017). Sensory and Physico‐psychological metaphor comprehension in children with ASD: A preliminary study on the outcomes of a treatment. Brain Sciences, 7(12), 85. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci7070085
  80. Melogno, S., Pinto, M. A., & Levi, G. (2012). Metaphor and metonymy in ASD children: A critical review from a developmental perspective. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 6(4), 1289–1296. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.rasd.2012.04.004
  81. Milosky, L. M., & Ford, J. A. (1997). The role of prosody in children's inferences of ironic intent. Discourse Processes, 23(1), 47–61. https://doi.org/10.1080/01638539709544981
  82. Morsanyi, K., & Stamenković, D. (2021). Idiom and proverb processing in autism: A systematic review and meta‐analysis. Journal of Cultural Cognitive Science, 5(3), 367–387. https://doi.org/10.1007/s41809-021-00079-4
  83. Morsanyi, K., Stamenković, D., & Holyoak, K. J. (2020). Metaphor processing in autism: A systematic review and meta‐analysis. Developmental Review, 57, 100925. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dr.2020.100925
  84. Nguyen, T.‐K., & Astington, J. W. (2014). Reassessing the bilingual advantage in theory of mind and its cognitive underpinnings. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 17, 396–409. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728913000394
  85. Nilsen, E. S., Glenwright, M., & Huyder, V. (2011). Children and adults understand that verbal irony interpretation depends on listener knowledge. Journal of Cognition and Development, 12(3), 374–409. https://doi.org/10.1080/15248372.2010.544693
  86. Norbury, C. F. (2004). Factors supporting idiom comprehension in children with communication disorders. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 47(5), 1179–1193. https://doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2004/087)
  87. Norbury, C. F. (2005). The relationship between theory of mind and metaphor: Evidence from children with language impairment and autistic spectrum disorder. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 23, 383–399. https://doi.org/10.1348/026151005X26732
  88. Osterhaus, C., & Bosacki, S. L. (2022). Looking for the lighthouse: A systematic review of advanced theory‐of‐mind tests beyond preschool. Developmental Review, 64, 101021. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dr.2022.101021
  89. Panzeri, F., Mazzaggio, G., Giustolisi, B., Silleresi, S., & Surian, L. (2022). The atypical pattern of irony comprehension in autistic children. Applied PsychoLinguistics, 43(4), 757–784. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0142716422000091
  90. Pexman, P., Reggin, L., & Lee, K. (2019). Addressing the challenge of verbal irony: Getting serious about sarcasm training. Languages, 4(2), 23. https://doi.org/10.3390/languages4020023
  91. Pexman, P. M., Rostad, K. R., McMorris, C. A., Climie, E. A., Stowkowy, J., & Glenwright, M. R. (2011). Processing of ironic language in children with high‐functioning autism Spectrum disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 41(8), 1097–1112. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-010-1131-7
  92. Pexman, P. M., Zdrazilova, L., McConnachie, D., Deater‐Deckard, K., & Petrill, S. A. (2009). “That was smooth, mom”: Children's production of verbal and gestural irony. Metaphor and Symbol, 24(4), 237–248. https://doi.org/10.1080/10926480903310286
  93. Rapp, A. M., & Wild, B. (2011). Nonliteral language in Alzheimer dementia: A review. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 17(2), 207–218. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1355617710001682
  94. Recchia, H. E., Howe, N., Ross, H. S., & Alexander, S. (2010). Children's understanding and production of verbal irony in family conversations. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 28(2), 255–274. https://doi.org/10.1348/026151008X401903
  95. Rivière, E., Klein, M., & Champagne‐Lavau, M. (2018). Using context and prosody in irony understanding: Variability amongst individuals. Journal of Pragmatics, 138, 165–172. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pragma.2018.10.006
  96. Roberts, R. M., & Kreuz, R. J. (1994). Why do people use figurative language? Psychological Science, 5(3), 159–163. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.1994.tb00653.x
  97. Rosello, B., Berenguer, C., Baixauli, I., García, R., & Miranda, A. (2020). Theory of mind profiles in children with autism Spectrum disorder: Adaptive/social skills and pragmatic competence. Frontiers in Psychology, 11, 567401. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2020.567401
  98. Rundblad, G., & Annaz, D. (2010). The atypical development of metaphor and metonymy comprehension in children with autism. Autism: The International Journal of Research and Practice, 14(1), 29–46. https://doi.org/10.1177/1362361309340667
  99. Rutherford, M. D., Baron‐Cohen, S., & Wheelwright, S. (2002). Reading the mind in the voice: A study with normal adults and adults with Asperger syndrome and high functioning autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 32(3), 189–194. https://doi.org/10.1023/a:1015497629971
  100. Saban‐Bezalel, R., Dolfin, D., Laor, N., & Mashal, N. (2019). Irony comprehension and mentalizing ability in children with and without autism Spectrum disorder. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 58, 30–38. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.rasd.2018.11.006
  101. Saban‐Bezalel, R., & Mashal, N. (2015). Hemispheric processing of idioms and irony in adults with and without pervasive developmental disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 45(11), 3496–3508. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-015-2496-4
  102. Saban‐Bezalel, R., & Mashal, N. (2018). Figurative language comprehension and laterality in autism Spectrum disorder. In O. Vartanian & R. E. Jung (Eds.), The Cambridge handbook of the neuroscience of creativity (pp. 281–296). Cambridge University Press. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781316556238.017
  103. Saban‐Bezalel, R., & Mashal, N. (2019). Different factors predict idiom comprehension in children and adolescents with ASD and typical development. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 49(12), 4740–4750. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-019-04193-9
  104. Schapira, C. (1999). Les stéréotypes en français: Proverbes et autres formules /. Ophrys, 92, 57.
  105. Selimis, S., & Katis, D. (2010). The emergence of nonliterality in spontaneous child speech: The case of Greek and English motion verbs. In S. Rice & J. Newman (Eds.), Experimental & empirical methods in cognitive/functional research (pp. 127–142). CSLI Publications.
  106. Semrud‐Clikeman, M., & Glass, K. (2010). The relation of humor and child development: Social, adaptive, and emotional aspects. Journal of Child Neurology, 25, 1248–1260. https://doi.org/10.1177/0883073810373144
  107. Silleresi, S., Prévost, P., Zebib, R., Bonnet‐Brilhault, F., Conte, D., & Tuller, L. (2020). Identifying language and cognitive profiles in children with ASD via a cluster analysis exploration: Implications for the new ICD‐11. Autism Research, 13(7), 1155–1167. https://doi.org/10.1002/aur.2268
  108. Steen, G. J., Dorst, A. G., Herrmann, J. B., Kaal, A. A., Krennmayr, T., & Pasma, T. (2010). A method for linguistic metaphor identification. From MIP to MIPVU.
  109. Sullivan, K., Winner, E., & Hopfield, N. (1995). How children tell a lie from a joke: The role of second‐order mental state attributions. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 13(2), 191–204. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-835X.1995.tb00673.x
  110. Sun, I. Y. I., Varanda, C. A., & Fernandes, F. D. (2017). Stimulation of executive functions as part of the language intervention process in children with autism Spectrum disorder. Folia Phoniatrica et Logopaedica: Official Organ of the International Association of Logopedics and Phoniatrics (IALP), 69(1–2), 78–83. https://doi.org/10.1159/000479586
  111. Swinney, D. A., & Cutler, A. (1979). The access and processing of idiomatic expressions. Journal of Verbal Learning & Verbal Behavior, 18(5), 523–534. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0022-5371(79)90284-6
  112. Tager‐Flusberg, H. (2000). Language and understanding minds: Connections in autism. In Understanding other minds: Perspectives from developmental cognitive neuroscience (2nd ed., pp. 124–149). Oxford University Press.
  113. Tager‐Flusberg, H., & Joseph, R. M. (2003). Identifying neurocognitive phenotypes in autism. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological Sciences, 358(1430), 303–314. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2002.1198
  114. Titone, D. A., & Connine, C. M. (1994). Comprehension of idiomatic expressions: Effects of predictability and literality. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 20(5), 1126–1138. https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.20.5.1126
  115. Tzuriel, D., & Groman, T. (2017). Dynamic assessment of figurative language of children in the autistic Spectrum: The relation to some cognitive and language aspects. Journal of Cognitive Education and Psychology, 16(1), 38–63. https://doi.org/10.1891/1945-8959.16.1.38
  116. Van Herwegen, J., & Rundblad, G. (2018). A cross‐sectional and longitudinal study of novel metaphor and metonymy comprehension in children, adolescents, and adults with autism Spectrum disorder. Frontiers in Psychology, 9, 945. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00945
  117. Vogindroukas, I., & Zikopoulou, O. (2011). Idiom understanding in people with Asperger syndrome/high functioning autism. Revista Da Sociedade Brasileira de Fonoaudiologia, 16(4), 390–395. https://doi.org/10.1590/S1516-80342011000400005
  118. Volden, J., Coolican, J., Garon, N., White, J., & Bryson, S. (2009). Brief report: Pragmatic language in autism spectrum disorder: Relationships to measures of ability and disability. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 39(2), 388–393. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-008-0618-y
  119. Vulchanova, M., Saldaña, D., Chahboun, S., & Vulchanov, V. (2015). Figurative language processing in atypical populations: The ASD perspective. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 9, 24. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2015.00024
  120. Vulchanova, M., Talcott, J. B., Vulchanov, V., Stankova, M., & Eshuis, H. (2012). Morphology in autism spectrum disorders: Local processing bias and language. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 29(7–8), 584–600. https://doi.org/10.1080/02643294.2012.762350
  121. Vulchanova, M., & Vulchanov, V. (2022). Rethinking figurative language in autism: What evidence can we use for interventions? Frontiers in Communication, 7. https://doi.org/10.3389/fcomm.2022.910850
  122. Vulchanova, M., Vulchanov, V., & Stankova, M. (2011). Idiom comprehension in the first language: A developmental study. Vigo International Journal of Applied Linguistics, 8, 206–234.
  123. Wang, A. T., Lee, S. S., Sigman, M., & Dapretto, M. (2006). Neural basis of irony comprehension in children with autism: The role of prosody and context. Brain: A Journal of Neurology, 129(Pt 4), 932–943. https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/awl032
  124. Whalen, J. M., & Pexman, P. M. (2010). How do children respond to verbal irony in face‐to‐face communication? The development of mode adoption across middle childhood. Discourse Processes, 47(5), 363–387. https://doi.org/10.1080/01638530903347635
  125. Whyte, E. M., & Nelson, K. E. (2015). Trajectories of pragmatic and nonliteral language development in children with autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Communication Disorders, 54, 2–14. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jcomdis.2015.01.001
  126. Whyte, E. M., Nelson, K. E., & Scherf, K. S. (2014). Idiom, syntax, and advanced theory of mind abilities in children with autism Spectrum disorders. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 57(1), 120–130. https://doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2013/12-0308)
  127. Williams, D. L., Cherkassky, V. L., Mason, R. A., Keller, T. A., Minshew, N. J., & Just, M. A. (2013). Brain function differences in language processing in children and adults with autism. Autism Research: Official Journal of the International Society for Autism Research, 6(4), 288–302. https://doi.org/10.1002/aur.1291
  128. Wilson, D., & Sperber, D. (2012). Meaning and relevance. Cambridge University Press. https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9781139028370
  129. Wimmer, H., & Perner, J. (1983). Beliefs about beliefs: Representation and constraining function of wrong beliefs in young children's understanding of deception. Cognition, 13(1), 103–128. https://doi.org/10.1016/0010-0277(83)90004-5
  130. Winner, E. (1988). The point of words: Children's understanding of metaphor and irony (pp. ix–212). Harvard University Press.
  131. Zheng, Q., Jia, Z., & Liang, D. (2015). Metaphor and metonymy comprehension in Chinese‐speaking children with high‐functioning autism. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 10, 51–58. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.rasd.2014.11.007

Grants

  1. /The research project entitled "Autism, Theory of Mind and Bilingualism (AuTism)" is implemented in the framework of H.F.R.I call "Basic research Financing (Horizontal support of all Sciences)" under the National Recovery and Resilience Plan "Greece 2.0" funded by the European Union -NextGeneration EU (H.F.R.I. Project Number: 14942, Principal Investigator: Maria Andreou).

MeSH Term

Child
Humans
Autism Spectrum Disorder
Language Tests
Language
Cognition
Autistic Disorder
Comprehension

Word Cloud

Created with Highcharts 10.0.0languageASDfigurativecomprehensionskillsprocessingnonliteralstudiesToMpragmaticsabilityresultsperformanceTheoryMindfunctionsEFsautisticreviewlightverbalfoundautismspectrumImpairmentsbroaderdomainconsidereddefiningfeatureAutismSpectrumDisorderschallengingaspectpragmaticcompetenceprocessInterestinglypreviousdemonstratedconflictingregardingparticipants'mainscientificdebatefocusesunderlyingfacilitatespeechNamelyabilitiesExecutiveregardedfactorsaffectingindividuals'addressesthreeinterpretiveaccountsrevieweddatarecentfieldconcludingchildrenindeedencountersystematicdifficultiesnon-literalMoreovercorrelateNotablydifferencesrelatedresearchmethodologytasks'propertiesmayleddiscrepanciesstudies'FinallyarguefutureencompassexperimentaldesigntasksminimallinguisticdemandsalsomeasuresordershedindependentcontributionFigurativedisorders:disorderexecutive

Similar Articles

Cited By