In treatments based on differential reinforcement of alternative behavior, applied researchers and clinicians often provide multiple, qualitatively different reinforcers (i.e., synthesized reinforcement) rather than a single reinforcer (i.e., isolated reinforcement) contingent on alternative behavior. Some research shows that providing synthesized reinforcement for alternative responses within such treatments produces more rapid and complete suppression of target behavior; however, there is limited research evaluating the durability of these effects during treatment disruptions. Conceptual explanations of resurgence (e.g., resurgence as choice, context theory) suggest that treatments that include synthesized alternative reinforcement may lead to more resurgence of target behavior when alternative reinforcement is disrupted relative to treatments using isolated reinforcement. We evaluated this hypothesis within a three-phase resurgence evaluation. We exposed rats to isolated or synthesized reinforcement for alternative responding in the second phase, and we exposed rats to extinction in the third phase. Synthesized alternative reinforcement produced more rapid and complete suppression of target behavior than did isolated reinforcement in the second phase; however, exposure to extinction following synthesized reinforcement produced more resurgence. We discuss these results in terms of their implications for applied research and their support for current conceptual explanations for resurgence.
Athens, E. S., & Vollmer, T. R. (2010). An investigation of differential reinforcement of alternative behavior without extinction. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 43(4), 569–589. https://doi.org/10.1901/jaba.2010.43-569
Baum, W. M., & Rachlin, H. C. (1969). Choice as time allocation. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 12(6), 861–874. https://doi.org/10.1901/jeab.1969.12-861
Balooch, S. B., & Neumann, D. L. (2011). Effects of multiple contexts and context similarity on the renewal of extinguished conditioned behaviour in an ABA design with humans. Learning and Motivation, 42(1), 53–63. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.lmot.2010.08.008
Bouton, M. E. (2002). Context, ambiguity, and unlearning: Sources of relapse after behavioral extinction. Biological Psychiatry, 52(10), 976–986. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0006-3223(02)01546-9
Bouton, M. E. (2004). Context and behavioral processes in extinction. Learning & Memory, 11(5), 485–494. https://doi.org/10.1101/lm.78804
Bouton, M. E., & Swartzentruber, D. (1991). Sources of relapse after extinction in Pavlovian and instrumental learning. Clinical Psychology Review, 11(2), 123–140. https://doi.org/10.1016/0272-7358(91)90091-8
Bouton, M. E., & Todd, T. P. (2014). A fundamental role for context in instrumental learning and extinction. Behavioural Processes, 104, 13–19. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.beproc.2014.02.012
Bouton, M. E., Winterbauer, N. E., & Todd, T. P. (2012). Relapse processes after the extinction of instrumental learning: Renewal, resurgence, and reacquisition. Behavioural Processes, 90(1), 130–141. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.beproc.2012.03.004
Briggs, A. M., Dozier, C. L., Lessor, A. N., Kamana, B. U., & Jess, R. L. (2019). Further investigation of differential reinforcement of alternative behavior without extinction for escape‐maintained destructive behavior. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 52(4), 956–973. https://doi.org/10.1002/jaba.648
Briggs, A. M., Fisher, W. W., Greer, B. D., & Kimball, R. T. (2018). Prevalence of resurgence of destructive behavior when thinning reinforcement schedules during functional communication training. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 51(3), 620–633. https://doi.org/10.1002/jaba.472
Brown, K. R., Greer, B. D., Craig, A. R., Sullivan, W. E., Fisher, W. W., & Roane, H. S. (2020). Resurgence following differential reinforcement of alternative behavior implemented with and without extinction. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 113(2), 449–467. https://doi.org/10.1002/jeab.588
Call, N. A., Wacker, D. P., Ringdahl, J. E., & Boelter, E. W. (2005). Combined antecedent variables as motivating operations within functional analyses. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 38(3), 385–389. https://doi.org/10.1901/jaba.2005.51-04
Craig, A. R., Browning, K. O., Nall, R. W., Marshall, C. M., & Shahan, T. A. (2017). Resurgence and alternative‐reinforcer magnitude. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 107(2), 218–233. https://doi.org/10.1002/jeab.245
Craig, A. R., & Shahan, T. A. (2016). Behavioral momentum theory fails to account for the effects of reinforcement rate on resurgence. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 105(3), 375–392. https://doi.org/10.1002/jeab.207
Devenport, L. D., & Devenport, J. A. (1994). Time‐dependent averaging of foraging information in least chipmunks and golden‐mantled ground squirrels. Animal Behaviour, 47(4), 787–802. https://doi.org/10.1006/anbe.1994.1111
Dolezal, D. N., & Kurtz, P. F. (2010). Evaluation of combined‐antecedent variables on functional analysis results and treatment of problem behavior in a school setting. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 43(2), 309–314. https://doi.org/10.1901/jaba.2010.43-309
Falligant, J. M., Chin, M. D., & Kurtz, P. F. (2022). Renewal and resurgence of severe problem behavior in an intensive outpatient setting: Prevalence, magnitude, and implications for practice. Behavioral Interventions, 37(3), 909–924. https://doi.org/10.1002/bin.1878
Falligant, J. M., Hagopian, L. P., Kranak, M. P., & Kurtz, P. F. (2022). Quantifying increases in problem behavior following downshifts in reinforcement: A retrospective analysis and replication. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 118(1), 148–155. https://doi.org/10.1002/jeab.769
Fisher, W. W., & Bouxsein, K. (2011). Developing function‐based reinforcement procedures for problem behavior. In W. W. Fisher, C. C. Pizza, & H. S. Roane (Eds.), Handbook of applied behavior analysis (2nd ed., pp. 337–351). Guilford.
Fisher, W. W., Greer, B. D., Mitteer, D. R., & Fuhrman, A. M. (2022). Translating quantitative theories of behavior into improved clinical treatments for problem behavior. Behavioural Processes, 198, Article 104639. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.beproc.2022.104639
Fisher, W. W., Greer, B. D., Romani, P. W., Zangrillo, A. N., & Owen, T. M. (2016). Comparisons of synthesized and individual reinforcement contingencies during functional analysis. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 49(3), 596–616. https://doi.org/10.1002/jaba.314
Fisher, W. W., Greer, B. D., Shahan, T. A., & Norris, H. M. (2023). Basic and applied research on extinction bursts. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 56(1), 4–28. https://doi.org/10.1002/jaba.954
Fleshler, M., & Hoffman, H. S. (1962). A progression for generating variable–interval schedules. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 5(4), 529–530. https://doi.org/10.1901/jeab.1962.5-529
Ghaemmaghami, M., Hanley, G. P., & Jessel, J. (2021). Functional communication training: From efficacy to effectiveness. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 54(1), 122–143. https://doi.org/10.1002/jaba.762
Ghaemmaghami, M., Hanley, G. P., Jin, S. C., & Vanselow, N. R. (2016). Affirming control by multiple reinforcers via progressive treatment analysis. Behavioral Interventions, 31(1), 70–86. https://doi.org/10.1002/bin.1425
Greer, B. D., Fisher, W. W., Saini, V., Owen, T. M., & Jones, J. K. (2016). Functional communication training during reinforcement schedule thinning: An analysis of 25 applications. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 49(1), 105–121. https://doi.org/10.1002/jaba.265
Hanley, G. P., Jin, C. S., Vanselow, N. R., & Hanratty, L. A. (2014). Producing meaningful improvements in problem behavior of children with autism via synthesized analyses and treatments. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 47(1), 16–36. https://doi.org/10.1002/jaba.106
Havermans, R. C., Keuker, J., Lataster, T., & Jansen, A. (2005). Contextual control of extinguished conditioned performance in humans. Learning and Motivation, 36(1), 1–19. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.lmot.2004.09.002
Hursh, S. R., Madden, G. J., Spiga, R., DeLeon, I. G., & Francisco, M. T. (2013). The translational utility of behavioral economics: The experimental analysis of consumption and choice. In G. J. Madden, W. V. Dube, T. D. Hackenberg, G. P. Hanley, & K. A. Lattal (Eds.), APA handbook of behavior analysis, vol. 2, Translating principles into practice (pp. 191–224). American Psychological Association. 10.1037/13938‐008
Irwin Helvey, C., Fisher, W. W., Greer, B. D., Fuhrman, A. M., & Mitteer, D. R. (2023). Resurgence of destructive behavior following differential rates of alternative reinforcement. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 56(4), 804–815. https://doi.org/10.1002/jaba.1010
Irwin Helvey, C., & Van Camp, C. M. (2022). Further comparison of isolated and synthesized contingencies in functional analyses. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 55(1), 154–168. https://doi.org/10.1002/jaba.890
Jarmolowicz, D. P., & Lattal, K. A. (2014). Resurgence under delayed reinforcement. The Psychological Record, 64(2), 189–193. https://doi.org/10.1007/s40732-014-0040-0
Jessel, J., Ingvarsson, E. T., Metras, R., Kirk, H., & Whipple, R. (2018). Achieving socially significant reductions in problem behavior following the interview‐informed synthesized contingency analysis: A summary of 25 outpatient applications. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 51(1), 130–157. https://doi.org/10.1002/jaba.436
Jones, S. H., & St. Peter, C. C. (2022). Nominally acceptable integrity failures negatively affect interventions involving intermittent reinforcement. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 55(4), 1109–1123. https://doi.org/10.1002/jaba.944
Kranak, M. P., & Falligant, J. M. (2021). Further investigation of resurgence following schedule thinning: Extension to an inpatient setting. Behavioral Interventions, 36(4), 1003–1012. https://doi.org/10.1002/bin.1831
Lattal, K. A., Cançado, C. R., Cook, J. E., Kincaid, S. L., Nighbor, T. D., & Oliver, A. C. (2017). On defining resurgence. Behavioural Processes, 141(1), 85–91. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.beproc.2017.04.018
Leon, Y., Wilder, D. A., Majdalany, L., Myers, K., & Saini, V. (2014). Errors of omission and commission during alternative reinforcement of compliance: The effects of varying levels of treatment integrity. Journal of Behavioral Education, 23, 19–33. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10864-013-9181-5
Mazur, J. E. (1996). Past experience, recency, and spontaneous recovery in choice behavior. Animal Learning & Behavior, 24(1), 1–10. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03198948
Mitteer, D. R., Greer, B. D., Fisher, W. W., Briggs, A. M., & Wacker, D. P. (2018). A laboratory model for evaluating relapse of undesirable caregiver behavior. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 110(2), 252–266. https://doi.org/10.1002/jeab.462
Mitteer, D. R., Greer, B. D., Randall, K. R., & Haney, S. D. (2022). On the scope and characteristics of relapse when treating severe destructive behavior. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 55(3), 688–703. https://doi.org/10.1002/jaba.912
Mitteer, D. R., Greer, B. D., Randall, K. R., Kimball, R. T., & Smith, S. W. (2021). Empirically deriving omission and commission errors for relapse tests: A demonstration of reverse translation. Behavior Analysis: Research and Practice, 21(4), 351–363. https://doi.org/10.1037/bar0000218
Muething, C., Pavlov, A., Call, N., Ringdahl, J., & Gillespie, S. (2021). Prevalence of resurgence during thinning of multiple schedules of reinforcement following functional communication training. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 54(2), 813–823. https://doi.org/10.1002/jaba.791
Oliver, A. C., Nighbor, T. D., & Lattal, K. A. (2018). Reinforcer magnitude and resurgence. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 110(3), 440–450. https://doi.org/10.1002/jeab.481
Retzlaff, B. J., Fisher, W. W., Akers, J. S., & Greer, B. D. (2020). A translational evaluation of potential iatrogenic effects of single and combined contingencies during functional analysis. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 53(1), 67–81. https://doi.org/10.1002/jaba.595
Ritchey, C. M., Kuroda, T., & Podlesnik, C. A. (2023). A quantitative analysis of resurgence following downshifts in alternative‐reinforcer magnitude. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 119(3), 501–512. https://doi.org/10.1002/jeab.843
Santiago, J. L., Hanley, G. P., Moore, K., & Jin, C. S. (2016). The generality of interview‐informed functional analyses: Systematic replications in school and home. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 46(3), 797–811. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-015-2617-0
Shahan, T. A., Browning, K. O., & Nall, R. W. (2020). Resurgence as choice in context: Treatment duration and on/off alternative reinforcement. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 113(1), 57–76. https://doi.org/10.1002/jeab.563
Shahan, T. A., Browning, K. O., Nist, A. N., & Sutton, G. M. (2020). Resurgence and downshifts in alternative reinforcement rate. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 114(2), 163–178. https://doi.org/10.1002/jeab.625
Shahan, T. A., & Craig, A. R. (2017). Resurgence as choice. Behavioural Processes, 141, 100–127. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.beproc.2016.10.006
Shahan, T. A., & Greer, B. D. (2021). Destructive behavior increases as a function of reductions in alternative reinforcement during schedule thinning: A retrospective quantitative analysis. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 116(2), 243–248. https://doi.org/10.1002/jeab.708
Shahan, T. A., Sutton, G. M., Van Allsburg, J., Avellaneda, M., & Greer, B. D. (2024). Resurgence following higher or lower quality alternative reinforcement. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 121(2), 246–258. https://doi.org/10.1002/jeab.904
Slaton, J. D., & Hanley, G. P. (2018). Nature and scope of synthesis in functional analysis and treatment of problem behavior. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 51(4), 943–973. https://doi.org/10.1002/jaba.498
Slaton, J. D., Hanley, G. P., & Raftery, K. J. (2017). Interview‐informed functional analyses: A comparison of synthesized and isolated components. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 50(2), 252–277. https://doi.org/10.1002/jaba.384
Smith, B. M., Smith, G. S., Shahan, T. A., Madden, G. J., & Twohig, M. P. (2017). Effects of differential rates of alternative reinforcement on resurgence of human behavior. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 107(1), 191–202. https://doi.org/10.1002/jeab.241
St. Peter Pipkin, C., Vollmer, T. R., & Sloman, K. N. (2010). Effects of treatment integrity failures during differential reinforcement of alternative behavior: A translational model. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 43(1), 47–70. https://doi.org/10.1901/jaba.2010.43-47
Sweeney, M. M., & Shahan, T. A. (2013). Effects of high, low, and thinning rates of alternative reinforcement on response elimination and resurgence. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 100(1), 102–116. https://doi.org/10.1002/jeab.26
Thomas, B. L., Larsen, N., & Ayres, J. J. (2003). Role of context similarity in ABA, ABC, and AAB renewal paradigms: Implications for theories of renewal and for treating human phobias. Learning and Motivation, 34(4), 410–436. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0023-9690(03)00037-7
Todd, T. P., Winterbauer, N. E., & Bouton, M. E. (2012). Effects of the amount of acquisition and contextual generalization on the renewal of instrumental behavior after extinction. Learning & Behavior, 40(2), 145–157. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13420-011-0051-5
Trask, S., Schepers, S. T., & Bouton, M. E. (2015). Context change explains resurgence after the extinction of operant behavior. Mexican Journal of Behavior Analysis, 41(2), 187–210. https://doi.org/10.5514/rmac.v41.i2.63772
Tsami, L., & Lerman, D. C. (2020). Transfer of treatment effects from combined to isolated conditions during functional communication training for multiply controlled problem behavior. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 53(2), 649–664. https://doi.org/10.1002/jaba.629
Vollmer, T. R., Peters, K. P., Kronfli, F. R., Lloveras, L. A., & Ibañez, V. F. (2020). On the definition of differential reinforcement of alternative behavior. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 53(3), 1299–1303. https://doi.org/10.1002/jaba.701
Vollmer, T. R., Roane, H. S., Ringdahl, J. E., & Marcus, B. A. (1999). Evaluating treatment challenges with differential reinforcement of alternative behavior. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 32(1), 9–23. https://doi.org/10.1901/jaba.1999.32-9
Winterbauer, N. E., & Bouton, M. E. (2010). Mechanisms of resurgence of an extinguished instrumental behavior. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 36(3), 343–353. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0017365
Wood, B. K., Umbreit, J., Liaupsin, C. J., & Gresham, F. M. (2007). A treatment integrity analysis of function‐based intervention. Education and Treatment of Children, 30(4), 105–120. https://doi.org/10.1353/etc.2007