Using behavioural network mapping to investigate dyadic play in girls with congenital adrenal hyperplasia.

Adriene M Beltz, Christel M Portengen, Sheri A Berenbaum
Author Information
  1. Adriene M Beltz: Department of Psychology, University of Michigan, Ann Arbor, Michigan, USA. ORCID
  2. Christel M Portengen: Department of Psychology, University of Michigan, Ann Arbor, Michigan, USA.
  3. Sheri A Berenbaum: Departments of Psychology and Pediatrics, The Pennsylvania State University, University Park, Pennsylvania, USA.

Abstract

Examining mechanisms underlying sex differences in children's play styles, we studied girls with congenital adrenal hyperplasia (CAH) who provide a test of the relative effects of prenatal androgens versus rearing, and of behavioural similarity versus gender identity and cognitions. In this exploratory study, 40 focal children (girls and boys with and without CAH), aged 3-8 years, played for 14 min with a same-sex peer in a task designed to elicit rough-and-tumble play. Time-indexed ratings of positive affect and vigour of activity were evaluated via network mapping for sex-related differences in both levels and play dynamics (temporal relations among behaviours). Results suggest influences of both gender identity-aligned social cognitions and prenatal androgens: there was greater dyadic synchrony between positive affect for girls (regardless of CAH status) than boys, but girls with CAH displayed positive affect levels and directed vigorous peer play dynamics similar to boys.

Keywords

References

  1. Beltz, A. M., Beekman, C., Molenaar, P. C., & Buss, K. A. (2013). Mapping temporal dynamics in social interactions with unified structural equation modeling: A description and demonstration revealing time‐dependent sex differences in play behavior. Applied Developmental Science, 17(3), 152–168. https://doi.org/10.1080/10888691.2013.805953
  2. Beltz, A. M., & Gates, K. M. (2017). Network mapping with GIMME. Multivariate Behavioral Research, 52(6), 789–804. https://doi.org/10.1080/00273171.2017.1373014
  3. Beltz, A. M., & Molenaar, P. C. (2016). Dealing with multiple solutions in structural vector autoregressive models. Multivariate Behavioral Research, 51(2–3), 357–373. https://doi.org/10.1080/00273171.2016.1151333
  4. Berenbaum, S. A. (2018). Beyond pink and blue: The complexity of early androgen effects on gender development. Child Development Perspectives, 12(1), 58–64. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdep.12261
  5. Berenbaum, S. A., & Beltz, A. M. (2021). Evidence and implications from a natural experiment of prenatal androgen effects on gendered behavior. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 30(3), 202–210. https://doi.org/10.1177/0963721421998341
  6. Berenbaum, S. A., Beltz, A. M., Bryk, K., & McHale, S. (2018). Gendered peer involvement in girls with congenital adrenal hyperplasia: Effects of prenatal androgens, gendered activities, and gender cognitions. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 47, 915–929. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10508‐017‐1112‐4
  7. Berenbaum, S. A., Bryk, K. L. K., & Beltz, A. M. (2012). Early androgen effects on spatial and mechanical abilities: Evidence from congenital adrenal hyperplasia. Behavioral Neuroscience, 126, 86–96. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0026652
  8. Berenbaum, S. A., & Hines, M. (1992). Early androgens are related to childhood sex‐typed toy preferences. Psychological Science, 3(3), 203–206. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467‐9280.1992.tb00028.x
  9. Berenbaum, S. A., & Resnick, S. M. (1997). Early androgen effects on aggression in children and adults with congenital adrenal hyperplasia. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 22(7), 505–515. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0306‐4530(97)00049‐8
  10. Blakemore, J. E. O., Berenbaum, S. A., & Liben, L. S. (2009). Gender development. Psychology Press.
  11. Chaku, N., & Beltz, A. M. (2022). Using temporal network methods to reveal the idiographic nature of development. Advances in Child Development and Behavior, 62, 159–190. https://doi.org/10.1016/bs.acdb.2021.11.003
  12. Chaplin, T. M., & Aldao, A. (2013). Gender differences in emotion expression in children: A meta‐analytic review. Psychological Bulletin, 139(4), 735–765. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0030737
  13. Collaer, M. L., & Hines, M. (2020). No evidence for enhancement of spatial ability with elevated prenatal androgen exposure in congenital adrenal hyperplasia: A meta‐analysis. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 49(2), 395–411. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10508‐020‐01645‐7
  14. Davis, J. T. M., & Hines, M. (2020). How large are gender differences in toy preferences? A systematic review and meta‐analysis in toy preference research. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 49, 373–394. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10508‐019‐01624‐7
  15. DiPietro, J. A. (1981). Rough and tumble play: A function of gender. Developmental Psychology, 17(1), 50–58. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012‐1649.17.1.50
  16. Endendijk, J. J., Beltz, A. M., McHale, S. M., Bryk, K., & Berenbaum, S. A. (2016). Linking prenatal androgens to gender‐related attitudes, identity, and activities: Evidence from girls with congenital adrenal hyperplasia. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 45, 1807–1815. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10508‐016‐0693‐7
  17. Engberg, H., Möller, A., Hagenfeldt, K., Nordenskjöld, A., & Frisén, L. (2020). Identity, sexuality, and parenthood in women with congenital adrenal hyperplasia. Journal of Pediatric and Adolescent Gynecology, 33(5), 470–476. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpag.2020.05.005
  18. Fabes, R. A., & Eisenberg, N. (1998). Meta‐analyses of age and sex differences in children's and adolescents' prosocial behavior. In W. Damon (Ed.), Handbook of child psychology (Vol. 3: Social, emotional, and personality development, 5th ed.). Wiley. https://doi.org/10.1002/9780470147658
  19. Fabes, R. A., Martin, C. L., & Hanish, L. D. (2003). Young children's play qualities in same‐, other‐, and mixed‐sex peer groups. Child Development, 74(3), 921–932. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467‐8624.00576
  20. Gates, K. M., & Molenaar, P. C. (2012). Group search algorithm recovers effective connectivity maps for individuals in homogeneous and heterogeneous samples. NeuroImage, 63(1), 310–319. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.06.026
  21. Gates, K. M., Molenaar, P. C., Hillary, F. G., Ram, N., & Rovine, M. J. (2010). Automatic search for fMRI connectivity mapping: An alternative to granger causality testing using formal equivalences among SEM path modeling, VAR, and unified SEM. NeuroImage, 50(3), 1118–1125. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.12.117
  22. Hampson, E., & Rovet, J. F. (2015). Spatial function in adolescents and young adults with congenital adrenal hyperplasia: Clinical phenotype and implications for the androgen hypothesis. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 54, 60–70. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psyneuen.2015.01.022
  23. Hines, M., & Kaufman, F. R. (1994). Androgen and the development of human sex‐typical behavior: Rough‐and‐tumble play and sex of preferred playmates in children with congenital adrenal hyperplasia (CAH). Child Development, 65(4), 1042–1053. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467‐8624.1994.tb00801.x
  24. Hines, M., Pasterski, V., Spencer, D., Neufeld, S., Patalay, P., Hindmarsh, P. C., Hughes, I. A., & Acerini, C. L. (2016). Prenatal androgen exposure alters girls' responses to information indicating gender‐appropriate behaviour. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 371, 20150125. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2015.0125
  25. Kung, K. T. F., Louie, K., Spencer, D., & Hines, M. (2024). Prenatal androgen exposure and sex‐typical play behavior: A meta‐analysis of classic congenital adrenal hyperplasia studies. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 159, 105616. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2024.105616
  26. Lane, S. T., & Gates, K. M. (2017). Automated selection of robust individual‐level structural equation models for time series data. Structural Equation Modeling: A Multidisciplinary Journal, 24(5), 768–782. https://doi.org/10.1080/10705511.2017.1309978
  27. Lane, S. T., Gates, K. M., Pike, H. K., Beltz, A. M., & Wright, A. G. (2019). Uncovering general, shared, and unique temporal patterns in ambulatory assessment data. Psychological Methods, 24, 54–69. https://doi.org/10.1037/met0000192
  28. Leaper, C. (2015). Gender and social‐cognitive development. In R. M. Lerner (Ed.), Handbook of child psychology and developmental science (Vol. 2: Cognitive processes, 7th ed., pp. 806–853). Wiley. https://doi.org/10.1002/9781118963418.childpsy
  29. Leveroni, C. L., & Berenbaum, S. A. (1998). Early androgen effects on interest in infants: Evidence from children with congenital adrenal hyperplasia. Developmental Neuropsychology, 14, 321–340. https://doi.org/10.1080/87565649809540714
  30. Maccoby, E. E. (1986). Social groupings in childhood: There relation to prosocial and antisocial behavior in boys and girls. In D. Olweus, J. Block, & M. Radke‐Yarrow (Eds.), Development of antisocial and prosocial behavior: Theories, research and issues (pp. 263–384). Academic Press.
  31. Maccoby, E. E., & Jacklin, C. N. (1987). Gender segregation in childhood. Advances in Child Development and Behavior, 20, 239–287. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0065‐2407(08)60404‐8
  32. Martin, C. L., & Fabes, R. A. (2001). The stability and consequences of young children's same‐sex peer interactions. Developmental Psychology, 37, 431–446. https://doi.org/10.1037//0012‐1649.37.3.431
  33. Martin, C. L., Fabes, R. A., Hanish, L., Leonard, S., & Dinella, L. M. (2011). Experienced and expected similarity to same‐gender peers: Moving toward a comprehensive model of gender segregation. Sex Roles, 65, 421–434. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11199‐011‐0029‐y
  34. Martin, C. L., & Halverson, C. F. (1981). A schematic processing model of sex typing and stereotyping in children. Child Development, 52, 1119–1134. https://doi.org/10.2307/1129498
  35. Martin, C. L., Kornienko, O., Schaefer, D. R., Hanish, L. D., Fabes, R. A., & Goble, P. (2013). The role of sex of peers and gender‐typed activities in young children's peer affiliative networks: A longitudinal analysis of selection and influence. Child Development, 84, 921–937. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12032
  36. Meyer‐Bahlburg, H. F., Dolezal, C., Baker, S. W., Ehrhardt, A. A., & New, M. I. (2006). Gender development in women with congenital adrenal hyperplasia as a function of disorder severity. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 35, 667–684. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10508‐006‐9068‐9
  37. Moller, L. C., Hymel, S., & Rubin, K. H. (1992). Sex typing in play and popularity in middle childhood. Sex Roles, 26, 331–353. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00289916
  38. Pasterski, V., Geffner, M. E., Brain, C., Hindmarsh, P., Brook, C., & Hines, M. (2011). Prenatal hormones and childhood sex segregation: Playmate and play style preferences in girls with congenital adrenal hyperplasia. Hormones and Behavior, 59(4), 549–555. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.yhbeh.2011.02.007
  39. Pasterski, V., Hindmarsh, P., Geffner, M., Brook, C., Brain, C., & Hines, M. (2007). Increased aggression and activity level in 3‐to 11‐year‐old girls with congenital adrenal hyperplasia (CAH). Hormones and Behavior, 52(3), 368–374. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.yhbeh.2007.05.015
  40. Pasterski, V. L., Geffner, M. E., Brain, C., Hindmarsh, P., Brook, C., & Hines, M. (2005). Prenatal hormones and postnatal socialization by parents as determinants of male‐typical toy play in girls with congenital adrenal hyperplasia. Child Development, 76(1), 264–278. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467‐8624.2005.00843.x
  41. Pellegrini, A. D. (2002). Rough‐and‐tumble play from childhood to adolescence: Development and possible functions. In P. K. Smith & C. H. Hart (Eds.), Blackwell handbook of childhood social development (pp. 438–453). Blackwell.
  42. Servin, A., Nordenström, A., Larsson, A., & Bohlin, G. (2003). Prenatal androgens and gender‐typed behavior: A study of girls with mild and severe forms of congenital adrenal hyperplasia. Developmental Psychology, 39, 440–450. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012‐1649.39.3.440
  43. Sörbom, D. (1989). Model modification. Psychometrika, 54(3), 371–384. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF02294623
  44. Todd, B. K., Fischer, R. A., Di Costa, S., Roestorf, A., Harbour, K., Hardiman, P., & Barry, J. A. (2018). Sex differences in children's toy preferences: A systematic review, meta‐regression, and meta‐analysis. Infant and Child Development, 27, e2064. https://doi.org/10.1002/icd.2064
  45. Trautner, H. M. (1995). Boys' and girls' play behavior in same‐sex and opposite‐sex pairs. The Journal of Genetic Psychology, 156, 5–15. https://doi.org/10.1080/00221325.1995.9914801
  46. Wallen, K. (2005). Hormonal influences on sexually differentiated behavior in nonhuman primates. Frontiers in Neuroendocrinology, 26(1), 7–26. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.yfrne.2005.02.001
  47. Wallen, K. (2022). Prenatal steroid hormones and sex differences in juvenile rhesus macaque behavior. In D. P. VanderLaan & W. I. Wong (Eds.), Gender and sexuality development: Contemporary theory and research (pp. 39–72). Springer.
  48. Wong, W. I., Pasterski, V., Hindmarsh, P. C., Geffner, M. E., & Hines, M. (2013). Are there parental socialization effects on the sex‐typed behavior of individuals with congenital adrenal hyperplasia? Archives of Sexual Behavior, 42, 381–391. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10508‐012‐9997‐4
  49. Zucker, K. J., Bradley, S. J., Oliver, G., Blake, J., Fleming, S., & Hood, J. (1996). Psychosexual development of women with congenital adrenal hyperplasia. Hormones and Behavior, 30, 300–318. https://doi.org/10.1006/hbeh.1996.0038

Grants

  1. HD19644/NIH HHS
  2. /Penn State College of the Liberal Arts
  3. /Penn State Child Study Center

Word Cloud

Created with Highcharts 10.0.0playgirlsCAHgenderboyspeerpositiveaffectdifferencescongenitaladrenalhyperplasiaprenatalversusbehaviouralcognitionsnetworkmappinglevelsdynamicsdyadicExaminingmechanismsunderlyingsexchildren'sstylesstudiedprovidetestrelativeeffectsandrogensrearingsimilarityidentityexploratorystudy40focalchildrenwithoutaged3-8 yearsplayed14 minsame-sextaskdesignedelicitrough-and-tumbleTime-indexedratingsvigouractivityevaluatedviasex-relatedtemporalrelationsamongbehavioursResultssuggestinfluencesidentity-alignedsocialandrogens:greatersynchronyregardlessstatusdisplayeddirectedvigoroussimilarUsinginvestigatebiologicaldevelopmentobservationalbehaviourstatistics

Similar Articles

Cited By