The role of bidirectional naming in the emergence of analogical relations in children.

Tatiana Zhirnova, Caio F Miguel, Maria Clara Cordeiro
Author Information
  1. Tatiana Zhirnova: Department of Psychology, California State University, Sacramento, CA, USA.
  2. Caio F Miguel: Department of Psychology, California State University, Sacramento, CA, USA. ORCID
  3. Maria Clara Cordeiro: Institute for Behavioral Science, Endicott College. ORCID

Abstract

The role of bidirectional naming in the emergence of analogical relations was investigated in four typically developing children between the ages 5 and 7���years. All participants learned to tact both the categories (clothes, furniture, and vehicles) and relations (same and different) among nine stimuli. They were subsequently tested on analogical responding during which they were presented with two stimuli belonging to the same or different categories and asked to select the comparison that matched the sample. During the last analogy test, we asked participants to tell us why they selected a certain comparison. Relational tact training produced emergent analogical responding in two participants after exposure to relational listener tests, whereas the other two required direct training on baseline analogy relations. All participants met criterion during derived analogy tests in accordance with symmetry and transitivity. The results of this study suggest that participants passed analogy tests by relationally tacting the sample (i.e., speaker) and reacting to its product by selecting the correct comparison (i.e., listener). This supports and extends previous findings suggesting that children must also engage in behaviors consistent with bidirectional naming to respond accurately to analogy tasks.

Keywords

References

  1. Bartha, P. (2019). Analogy and analogical reasoning. In E. N. Zalta (Ed.), The Stanford Encyclopedia of Philosophy. The Metaphysics Research Lab, Center for the Study of Language and Information, Stanford University. https://plato.stanford.edu/archives/spr2019/entries/reasoning-analogy
  2. Barnes, D., Hegarty, N., & Smeets, P. M. (1997). Relating equivalence relations to equivalence relations: A relational framing model of complex human functioning. The Analysis of Verbal Behavior, 14(1), 57���83. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF03392916
  3. Carpentier, F., Smeets, P. M., & Barnes���Holmes, D. (2002). Matching functionally���same relations: Implications for equivalence���equivalence as a model for analogical reasoning. The Psychological Record, 52(3), 312���351. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF03395435
  4. Carpentier, F., Smeets, P. M., & Barnes���Holmes, D. (2003). Equivalence���equivalence as a model of analogy: Further analyses. The Psychological Record, 53(3), 349���371.
  5. Carpentier, F., Smeets, P. M., Barnes���Holmes, D., & Stewart, I. (2004). Matching derived functionally���same stimulus relations: Equivalence���equivalence and classical analogies. The Psychological Record, 54(2), 255���273. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF03395473
  6. Cordeiro, M. C., Zhirnova, T., & Miguel, C. F. (2020). Establishing equivalence���equivalence analogical relations via tact and listener training. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 115(1), 340���360. https://doi.org/10.1002/jeab.652
  7. Csap��, B. (1997). The development of inductive reasoning: Cross���sectional assessments in an educational context. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 20(4), 609���626. https://doi.org/10.1080/016502597385081
  8. Garcia, A. G., Gonzalea, A. V., & Lefkowitz, J. G. (2014). The influence of the parents' educational level and participants' age in the derivation of equivalence���equivalence. Psicothema, 26(3), 314���320. https://doi.org/10.7334/psicothema2013.215
  9. Green, G. (2001). Behavior analytic instruction for learners with autism: Advances in stimulus control technology. Focus on Autism and other Developmental Disabilities, 16(2), 72���85. https://doi.org/10.1177/108835760101600203
  10. Green, A. E., Kramer, D. J., Fugelsang, J. A., Gray, J. R., & Dunbar, K. N. (2010). Connecting long distance: Semantic distance in analogical reasoning modulates frontpolar cortex activity. Cerebral Cortex, 20(1), 70���76. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhp081
  11. Green, A. E., Kramer, D. J., Fugelsang, J. A., Gray J. R., & Dunbar, K. N. (2011). Neural correlates of creativity in analogical reasoning. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 38(2), 264���272. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0025764
  12. Goswami, U. (1991). Analogical reasoning: What develops? A review of research and theory. Child Development, 62(1), 1���22. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.1991.tb01511.x
  13. Goswami, U., & Brown, A. L. (1990). Higher order structure and relational reasoning. Contrasting analogical and thematic relations. Cognition, 36(3), 207���226. https://doi.org/10.1016/0010-0277(90)90057-Q
  14. Hosenfeld, B., Van den Boom, D. C., & Resing, W. C. M. (1997). Constructing geometric analogies for the longitudinal testing of elementary school children. Journal of Educational Measurement, 34(4) 367���372. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1745-3984.1997.tb00524.x
  15. Horne, P. J., Lowe, C. F., & Randle, V. R. L. (2004). Naming and categorization in young children: II. Listener behavior training. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 81(3), 267���288. https://doi.org/10.1901/jeab.2004.81-267
  16. Johnson, C., & Sidman, M. (1993). Conditional discrimination and equivalence relations: Control by negative stimuli. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 59(2), 333���347. https://doi.org/10.1901/jeab.1993.59-333
  17. Kobari���Wright, V. V., & Miguel, C. F. (2014). The effects of listener training on the emergence of categorization and speaker behavior in children with autism. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 47(2), 431���436. https://doi.org/10.1002/jaba.115
  18. Kuncel, N. R., Hezlett, S. A., & Ones, D. S. (2004). Academic performance, career potential, creativity, and job performance: Can one construct predict them all? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 86(1), 148���161. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.86.1.148
  19. Lantaya, C. A., Miguel, C. F., Howland, T. G., LaFrance, D. L., & Page, S. (2018). An evaluation of a visual���visual successive matching���to���sample procedure to establish equivalence classes in adults. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 109(3), 533���550. https://doi.org/10.1002/jeab.326
  20. Lee, G. P., Miguel, C. F., Darcey, E. K., & Jennings, A. M. (2015). A further evaluation of the effects of listener training on the emergence of speaker behavior and categorization in children with autism. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 19, 72���81. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.rasd.2015.04.007
  21. Levinson, P. J., & Carpenter, R. L. (1974) An analysis of analogical reasoning in children. Child Development, 45, 857���861. https://doi.org/10.2307/1127862
  22. Lowe, C. F., Horne, P. J., Harris, F. D. A., & Randle, V. R. L. (2002). Naming and categorization in young children: Vocal tact training. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 78(3), 527���549. https://doi.org/10.1901/jeab.2002.78-527
  23. Mayer, R. E. (1999). Fifty years of creativity research. In R. J. Sternberg (Ed.), Handbook of creativity (pp. 449���460). Cambridge University Press.
  24. Meyer, C., Cordeiro, M. C., & Miguel, C. F. (2019). The effects of listener training on the development of analogical reasoning. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 112(2), 144���166. https://doi.org/10.1002/jeab.549
  25. McLoughlin, S., Tyndall, I., & Pereira, A. (2020). Convergence of multiple fields on a relational reasoning approach to cognition. Intelligence, 83, Article 101491. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.intell.2020.101491
  26. Miguel, C. F. (2016). Common and intraverbal bidirectional naming. The Analysis of Verbal Behavior, 32(2), 125���138. https://doi.org/10.1007/s40616-016-0066-2
  27. Miguel, C. F. (2018). Problem���solving, bidirectional naming, and the development of verbal repertoires. Behavior Analysis: Research and Practice, 18(4), 340���353. https://doi.org/10.1037/bar0000110
  28. Miguel, C. F., Frampton, S. E., Lantaya, C. A., LaFrance, D. L., Quah, K., Meyer, C. C., Elias, N. C., & Fernand, J. K. (2015). The effects of tact training on the development of analogical reasoning. Journal of the Experimental Analysis, 104(2), 96���118. https://doi.org/10.1002/jeab.167
  29. Miguel, C. F., & Kobari���Wright, V. V. (2013). The effects of tact training on the emergence of categorization and listener behavior in children with autism. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 46(3), 669���773. https://doi.org/10.1002/jaba.62
  30. Miguel, C. F., Petursdottir, A. I., Carr, J. E., & Michael, J. (2008). The role of naming in stimulus categorization by preschool children. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 89(3), 383���405. https://doi.org/10.1901/jeab.2008-89-383
  31. Najdowski, A. (2016). Flexible and focused: Teaching executive function skills to individuals with autism and attention disorders. Academic Press.
  32. Piaget, J., Montangero, J., & Billeter, J. (1977). Les correlates. In J. Piaget (Ed.), L'abstraction Reflechissante. Presses Universitaires de France.
  33. Ribeiro, D. M., Cavalcante, R. S. T., Bandeira, M. T. S., Sella, A. C., & Miguel, C. F. (2018). Efeitos do ensino do tato na emerg��ncia da categoriza����o em crian��as com autismo. Acta Comportamentalia, 26(1), 71���91. https://doi.org/10.32870/ac.v26i1.63597
  34. Ribeiro, D. M., & Miguel, C. F. (2020). The use of multiple���tact training to produce emergent categorization in children with autism. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 53(3), 1768���1779. https://doi.org/10.1002/jaba.687
  35. Richland, L., Morrison, R. G., & Holyoak, K. J. (2006). Children's development of analogical reasoning: Insights from scene analogy problems. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 94, 249���273. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2006.02.002
  36. Richland, L., & Simms, N. (2015). Analogy, higher order thinking, and education. WIREs Cognitive Science, 6(2), 177���192. https://doi.org/10.1002/wcs.1336
  37. Ruiz, F. J., & Luciano, C. (2011). Cross���domain analogies as relating derived relations among two separate relational networks. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 95(3), 369���385. https://doi.org/10.1901/jeab.2011.95-369
  38. Sidman, M. (2009). Equivalence relations and behavior: An introductory tutorial. The Analysis of Verbal Behavior, 25, 1���13. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF03393066
  39. Simms, N. K., Frausel, R. R., & Richland, L. E. (2018). Working memory predicts children's analogical reasoning. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 166, 160���177. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2017.08.005
  40. Skinner, B. F. (1957). Verbal behavior. Copley Publishing Group.
  41. Skinner, B. F. (1984). An operant analysis of problem solving. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 7(4), 583���613. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X00027412
  42. Starr, A., Vendetti, M. S., & Bunge, S. A. (2018). Eye movements provide insight into individual differences in children's analogical reasoning strategies. Acta Psychologica, 186, 18���26. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.actpsy.2018.04.002
  43. Sternberg, R. J., & Rifkin, B. (1979). The development of analogical reasoning processes. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 27, 195���232. https://doi.org/10.1016/0022-0965(79)90044-4
  44. Stevenson, C. E., Heiser, W. J., & Resing, W. C. M. (2013). Working memory as a moderator of training and transfer of analogical reasoning in children. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 38(3), 159���169. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cedpsych.2013.02.001
  45. Stewart, I., Barnes���Holmes, D., Hayes, S. C., & Lipkens, R. (2001). Relations among relations: Analogies, metaphors, and stories. In S. C. Hayes, D. Barnes���Holmes, & B. Roche (Eds.), Relational frame theory: A post���Skinnerian account of language and cognition (pp. 73���86). Plenum.
  46. Stewart, I., Barnes���Homes, D., Roche, B., & Smeets, P. M. (2002). A functional���analytic model of analogy: A relational frame analysis. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 78(3), 375���396. https://doi.org/10.1901/jeab.2002.78-375
  47. Watson, P. J., & Workman, E. A. (1981). The nonconcurrent multiple baseline across individuals design: An extension of the traditional multiple baseline design. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 12(3), 257���259. https://doi.org/10.1016/0005-7916(81)90055-0

MeSH Term

Humans
Child
Male
Child, Preschool
Female
Concept Formation
Child Development

Word Cloud

Created with Highcharts 10.0.0analogicalparticipantsanalogybidirectionalnamingrelationschildrenrespondingtwocomparisontestsroleemergencetactcategoriesdifferentstimuliaskedsampletraininglistenerderivedieinvestigatedfourtypicallydevelopingages57���yearslearnedclothesfurniturevehiclesamongninesubsequentlytestedpresentedbelongingselectmatchedlasttesttellusselectedcertainRelationalproducedemergentexposurerelationalwhereasrequireddirectbaselinemetcriterionaccordancesymmetrytransitivityresultsstudysuggestpassedrelationallytactingspeakerreactingproductselectingcorrectsupportsextendspreviousfindingssuggestingmustalsoengagebehaviorsconsistentrespondaccuratelytasksreasoningstimulusequivalenceverbalbehavior

Similar Articles

Cited By

No available data.